Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-hxdxx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T16:58:06.182Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Session-effects in the laboratory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Guillaume R. Fréchette*
Affiliation:
New York University, New York, USA

Abstract

In experimental economics, where subjects participate in different sessions, observations across subjects of a given session might exhibit more correlation than observations across subjects in different sessions. The main goal of this paper is to clarify what are session-effects: what can cause them, what forms they can take, and what are the potential problems. It will be shown that standard solutions are at times inadequate, and that their properties are sometimes misunderstood.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2011 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

I wish to thank the editor and two anonymous referees, Marina Agranov, Jeffrey Carpenter, Gary Charness, Yan Chen, Vincent Crawford, Pedro Dal Bó, Mark Dean, John Duffy, Martin Dufwenberg, Drew Fudenberg, John Ham, Burkhard Schipper, Andrew Schotter, Chloé Tergiman, Kevin Thom, Isabel Treviño, and Emanuel Vespa for helpful comments and the NSF via grants SES-0519045, SES-0721111, and SES-0924780, and the Center for Experimental Social Science (CESS) for their financial support.

References

Altman, D. G., & Bland, J. M. (1997). Statistical notes: units of analysis. British Medical Journal, 314, 1874 10.1136/bmj.314.7098.1874CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aoyagi, M., & Fréchette, G. R. (2009). Collusion as public monitoring becomes noisy: experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Theory, 144(3), 11351165. 10.1016/j.jet.2008.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apesteguia, J., Huck, S., & Oechssler, J. (2007). Imitation-theory and experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Theory, 136, 217235. 10.1016/j.jet.2006.07.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arellano, M. (2003). Panel data econometrics, London: Oxford University Press 10.1093/0199245282.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armantier, O. (2004). Does observation influence learning?. Games and Economic Behavior, 46(2), 221239. 10.1016/S0899-8256(03)00124-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty, W. W. Gerall, A. A., Moltz, H., & Ward, I. L. (1992). Gonadal hormones and sex differences in nonreproductive behaviors. Handbook of behavioral neurology, New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Blume, A., Duffy, J., & Franco, A. M. (2009). Decentralized organizational learning: an experimental investigation. American Economic Review, 99(4), 11781205. 10.1257/aer.99.4.1178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C. F., Ho, T.-H., & Chong, J.-K. (2002). Sophisticated EWA learning and strategic teaching in repeated games. Journal of Economic Theory, 104, 137188. 10.1006/jeth.2002.2927CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, A. C., Gelbach, J. B., & Miller, D. L. (2008). Bootstrap-based improvements for inference with clustered errors. Review of Economics and Statistics, 90, 414427. 10.1162/rest.90.3.414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, G. (1980). Analysis of covariance with qualitative data. Review of Economic Studies, 47, 225238. 10.2307/2297110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G., Fréchette, G. R., & Kagel, J. H. (2004). How robust is laboratory gift exchange?. Experimental Economics, 7(2), 189205. 10.1023/B:EXEC.0000026979.14590.3cCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G., Fréchette, G. R., & Qin, C.-Z. (2007). Endogenous transfers in the prisoner’s dilemma game: an experimental test of cooperation and coordination. Games and Economic Behavior, 60(2), 287306. 10.1016/j.geb.2006.10.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Y., & Katuscak, P. Ozdenoren, E. (2005). Why can’t a woman bid more like a man. Mimeo.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R., DeJong, D. V., Forsythe, R., & Ross, T. W. (1996). Cooperation without reputation: experimental evidence from prisoner’s dilemma games. Games and Economic Behavior, 12, 187218. 10.1006/game.1996.0013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croson, R. (1996). Partners and strangers revisited. Economics Letters, 53, 2532. 10.1016/S0165-1765(97)82136-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabbs, James M. J. (1990). Salivary testosterone measurements: reliability across hours, days, and weeks. Physiology & Behavior, 48(1), 8386. 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90265-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, D. D., & Holt, C. A. (1993). Experimental economics, Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Excoffier, L., Smouse, P. E., & Quattro, J. M. (1992). Analysis of molecular variance inferred from metric distances among DNA haplotypes: application to human mitochondrial DNA restriction data. Genetics, 131, 479491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischbacher, U., Gächter, S., & Fehr, E. (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71, 397404. 10.1016/S0165-1765(01)00394-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fréchette, G. R. (2009). Learning in a multilateral bargaining experiments. Journal of Econometrics, 153(2), 183195. 10.1016/j.jeconom.2009.06.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fréchette, G. R., Kagel, J. H., & Morelli, M. (2005). Behavioral identification in coalitional bargaining: an experimental analysis of demand bargaining and alternating offers. Econometrica, 73(6), 18931938. 10.1111/j.1468-0262.2005.00641.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, D., & Sunder, S. (1994). Experimental methods: a primer for economists, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 10.1017/CBO9781139174176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fudenberg, D., & Levine, D. K. (1999). Learning and evolution in games, Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gneezy, U., Niederle, M., & Rustichini, A. (2003). Performance in competitive environments: gender differences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, CXVIII, 10491074. 10.1162/00335530360698496CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ham, J. C., Kagel, J. H., & Lehrer, S. F. (2005). Randomization, endogeneity and laboratory experiments: the role of cash balances in private value auctions. Journal of Econometrics, 125(1–2), 175205. 10.1016/j.jeconom.2004.04.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckman, J. J. (1981). Structural analysis of discrete data with econometric applications. The incidental parameters problem and the problem of initial conditions in estimating a discrete time-discrete data stochastic process, Cambridge: MIT Press 179195.Google Scholar
Hsiao, C. (2003). Analysis of panel data, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press 10.1017/CBO9780511754203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanova-Stenzel, R., & Salmon, T. C. (2008). Revenue equivalence revisited. Games and Economic Behavior, 64, 171192. 10.1016/j.geb.2008.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehrer, S. F., Tremblay, R. E., & Vitaro, F. Schaal, B. (2004). Raging hormones in puberty: do they influence adolescent risky behavior? Mimeo.Google Scholar
Lewejohann, L., Reinhard, C., Schrewe, A., Brandewiede, J., Haemisch, A., Görtz, N., Schachner, M., & Sachser, N. (2006). Environmental bias? Effects of housing conditions, laboratory environment and experimenter on behavioral tests. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 5, 6472. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00140.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, A. E., Prasnikar, V., Okuno-Fujiwara, M., & Zamir, S. (1991). Bargaining and market behavior in Jerusalem, Ljubljana, Pittsburgh, and Tokyo: an experimental study. American Economic Review, 81(5), 10681095.Google Scholar
Sakata, S. (2002). Quasi-maximum likelihood estimation with complex survey data. Mimeo.Google Scholar
Schipper, B. C. (2011). Sex hormones and choice under risk. Working Paper, UC Davis.Google Scholar
Seinen, I., & Schram, A. (2006). Social status and group norms: indirect reciprocity in a repeated helping experiment. European Economic Review, 50, 581602. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2004.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, K., & McClintock, M. K. (1998). Regulation of ovulation by human pheromones. Nature, 392(12), 177179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Huyck, J., & Battalio, R. C. Beil, R. (1991). Strategic uncertainty, equilibrium selection, and coordination failure in average opinion games. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 885910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanberg, C. (2008). Why do people keep their promises? An experimental test of two explanations. Econometrica, 76(6), 14671480. 10.3982/ECTA7673Google Scholar
Warton, D. I., & Hudson, H. M. (2004). A MANOVA statistic is just as powerful as distance-based statistics, for multivariate abundances. Ecology, 85(3), 858874. 10.1890/02-0419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, R. L. (2000). A note on robust variance estimation for cluster-correlated data. Biometrics, 56, 645646. 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00645.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wooldridge, J. M. (2002). Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data, Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar