Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-sk4tg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T16:02:09.338Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

My five pounds are not as good as yours, so I will spend them

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Sotiris Vandoros*
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE, UK

Abstract

An experimental approach is used to examine how money denomination and the choice between or availability of coins and banknotes may influence consumers’ purchasing behaviour. Evidence shows that for small amounts of money, consumers may prefer a smaller monetary value in banknotes rather than a higher value in coins. Findings also suggest that people carrying coins are more likely to make a purchase of small value than people not carrying coins.

JEL classification

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2013 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic Supplementary Material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-013-9351-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Easy on the mind, easy on the wallet: the roles of familiarity and processing fluency in valuation judgements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(5), 985990. 10.3758/PBR.15.5.985CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boustead, E., Cottee, K., Farquhar, R., Jonas, R., Walter, J., & Webley, P. (1992). The perceived value of a new coin. Journal of Social Psychology, 132, 143144. 10.1080/00224545.1992.9924700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, V., Gilmore, D., Mason, L., & Mayhew, P. (1983). Factors affecting the perceived value of coins. Journal of Economic Psychology, 4, 335347. 10.1016/0167-4870(83)90038-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and needs as organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 42(1), 3344. 10.1037/h0058484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cannon, E., & Cipriani, G. P. (2006). Euro-illusion: a natural experiment. Journal of Money, Credit, and Banking, 38(5), 13911403. 10.1353/mcb.2006.0066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coinstar. (2010) Frequently asked questions. http://www.coinstar.co.uk/uk/html/A5-3. Accessed 13 September 2010.Google Scholar
Furnham, A. (1983). Inflation and the estimated sizes of notes. Journal of Economic Psychology, 4, 349352. 10.1016/0167-4870(83)90039-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Office, General Accounting (1990). National coinage proposals. Limited public demand for new dollar coin or elimination of pennies. Report to Chairman, Subcommittee on Consumer Affairs and Coinage, Committee on Banking, Finance and Urban Affairs, House of Representatives. GAO/GGD-90-88.Google Scholar
Office, General Accounting (2002). New dollar coin. Marketing campaign raised public awareness but not widespread use. Report to the Subcommittee on Treasury and General Government, Committee on Appropriations, U.S. Senate. GAO-02-896.Google Scholar
Kohli, U. (1988). A note on banknote characteristics and the demand for currency by denomination. Journal of Banking & Finance, 12, 389399. 10.1016/0378-4266(88)90005-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kooreman, P., Faber, R. P., & Hofmans, H. M. J. (2004). Charity donations and the Euro introduction: some quasi-experimental evidence on money illusion. Journal of Money, Credit, and Banking, 36(6), 11211124. 10.1353/mcb.2005.0011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, S. E. G. (1981). Inflation, decimalization and the estimated sizes of coins. Journal of Economic Psychology, 1, 7981. 10.1016/0167-4870(81)90006-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, M., Wallace, N., & Zhu, T. (2005). Modelling denomination structures. Econometrica, 73(3), 949960. 10.1111/j.1468-0262.2005.00599.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manoj, T., Desai, K. K., & Seenivasan, S. (2011). How credit card payments increase unhealthy food purchases: visceral regulation of vices. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 126139. 10.1086/657331Google Scholar
Michell, H. (1947). The iron money of Sparta. Phoenix, 1 supplement, 4244. 10.2307/1086107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, H., Mishra, A., & Nayakankuppam, D. (2006). Money: a bias for the whole. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(4), 541549. 10.1086/500484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Council, Payments (2010a). The future of cash in the UK, London, March.Google Scholar
Council, Payments (2010b). The way we pay, London.Google Scholar
Raghubir, P., & Srivastava, J. (2009). The denomination effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(4), 701713. 10.1086/599222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raghubir, P., & Srivastava, J. (2008). Monopoly money: the effect of payment coupling and form on spending behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Applied, 14(3), 213225. 10.1037/1076-898X.14.3.213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Telser, L. G. (1995). Optimal denominations for coins and currency. Economics Letters, 49, 425427. 10.1016/0165-1765(95)00691-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Royal Mint. (2010). Coins designs and specifications. http://www.royalmint.com/Corporate/facts/coins/. Accessed 13 September 2010.Google Scholar
Van Hove, L. (2001). Optimal denominations for coins and banknotes: on defence of the principle of least effort. Journal of Money, Credit, and Banking, 33, 10151021. 10.2307/2673933CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hove, L., & Heyndels, B. (1996). On the optimal spacing of currency denominations. European Journal of Operational Research, 90, 547552. 10.1016/0377-2217(95)00072-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynne, M. A. (1997). More on optimal denominations for coins and currency. Economics Letters, 55, 221225. 10.1016/S0165-1765(97)00080-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Vandoros supplementary material

Online Appendix
Download Vandoros supplementary material(File)
File 120.4 KB