Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-dkgms Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-16T21:13:46.598Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How does voice matter? Evidence from the ultimatum game

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Qiyan Ong*
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, Nanyang Technological University, 14 Nanyang Drive, Singapore 637332, Singapore Economic Growth Centre, Nanyang Technological University, 14 Nanyang Drive, Singapore 637332, Singapore
Yohanes E. Riyanto*
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, Nanyang Technological University, 14 Nanyang Drive, Singapore 637332, Singapore
Steven M. Sheffrin*
Affiliation:
Murphy Institute, Tulane University, 108 Tilton Hall, New Orleans, LA 70115, USA

Abstract

Prior research has demonstrated that the ability to express one’s views or “voice” matters in social and economic interactions, but little is known of the mechanisms through which voice operates. Using an experimental approach based on the ultimatum game with the strategy method, we explore four potential channels for voice that encompass and expand on prior work: the knowledge effect of voice, the value expressive (or inherent value) of voice, the expectation effect of voice, and the procedural fairness effects of voice. Our results show strong effects through the value expressive and expectation channel, but not through either the knowledge channel or procedural fairness. In our view, voice is powerful because people like to express their views and they are disappointed when their views did not make a difference in their outcomes.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2012 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic Supplementary Material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-012-9316-x) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Brandts, J., & Charness, G. (2011). The strategy versus the direct-response method: a first survey of experimental comparisions. Experimental Economics, 14(3), 375398. 10.1007/s10683-011-9272-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolton, G., & Ockenfels, A. (2000). ERC: a theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. American Economic Review, 90(1), 166193. 10.1257/aer.90.1.166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, K., & Leben, S. (2007). Procedural fairness: a key ingredient in public satisfaction. Court Review, 44(1–2), 425.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. (2003). Behavioral game theory: experiments in strategic interaction, Princeton/New York: Princeton University Press/Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Crawford, V. (1998). A survey of experiments on communication via cheap talk. Journal of Economic Theory, 78(2), 286298. 10.1006/jeth.1997.2359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, P., Edlin, R., Tsuchiya, A., & Wailoo, A. (2007). It ain’t what you do, it’s the way that you do it: characteristics of procedural justice and their importance in social decision-making. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 64, 157170. 10.1016/j.jebo.2006.07.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. (1999). A Theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868. 10.1162/003355399556151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: combined impact of “voice” and improvement of experienced inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 108119. 10.1037/0022-3514.35.2.108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J., & Folger, R. Paulus, P. B. (1983). Procedural justice, participation and the fair process effect in groups and organizations. Basic group processes, New York: Springer 235256. 10.1007/978-1-4612-5578-9_10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. (1990). Organizational justice: yesterday, today and tomorrow. Journal of Management, 16, 399432. 10.1177/014920639001600208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habermas, J. (1970). Technology and science as ‘ideology’, Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Hirschman, A. (1970). Exit, voice, and loyalty: responses to decline in firms, organizations, and slates, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. (1996). On expectations and the monetary stakes in ultimatum games. International Journal of Game Theory, 25(3), 289302. 10.1007/BF02425259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houser, D., & Xiao, E. (2010). Classification of natural language messages using a coordination game. Experimental Economics, 14, 114. 10.1007/s10683-010-9254-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. Journal of Business, 59, 285300. 10.1086/296367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1991). Anomalies: the endowment effect, loss aversion and status quo bias. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5(1), 193206. 10.1257/jep.5.1.193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24, 163204. 10.1086/266945CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köszegi, B., & Rabin, M. (2006). A model of reference-dependent preferences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(4), 11331165.Google Scholar
Lind, E., Kurtz, S., Musante, L., Walker, L., & Thibaut, J. (1980). Procedural and outcome effects on reactions to adjudicated resolution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 643653. 10.1037/0022-3514.39.4.643CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lind, E., Kanfer, R., & Earley, P. (1990). Voice, control and procedural justice: instrumental and no instrumental concerns in fairness judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 952959. 10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.952CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lind, E., Kulik, C., Ambrose, M., & De Vera Park, M. (1993). Individual and corporate dispute resolution: Using procedural fairness as a decision heuristic. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 224251. 10.2307/2393412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
List, J., & Cherry, T. (2000). Learning to accept in the ultimatum game: evidence from an experimental design that generates low offers. Experimental Economics, 3(1), 1129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miceli, M. P., Lane, M. C. Rowland, K., & Ferris, G. R. (1991). Antecedents of pay satisfaction: a review and extension. Research in personnel and human resources management, Greenwich: JAI Press 235309.Google Scholar
Nelson, W. (2002). Equity or intention: it’s the thought that counts. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 48, 423430. 10.1016/S0167-2681(01)00245-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. (1999). Can you have too much of a good thing? The limits of voice for improving satisfaction with leaders. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 313324. 10.1177/0146167299025003004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, A., & Erev, I. (1995). Learning in extensive-form games: experimental data and simple dynamic models in the intermediate term. Games and Economic Behavior, 8, 164212. 10.1016/S0899-8256(05)80020-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selten, R., & Sauermann, H. (1967). Die Strategiemethode zur Erforschung des eingeschränkt rationale Verhaltens im Rahmen eines Oligopolexperiments. Beiträge zur experimentellen Wirtschaftsforschung, Tübingen: Mohr 136168.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. (1988). Anomalies: the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 2, 195206. 10.1257/jep.2.4.195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: a psychological analysis, Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tyler, T., Rasinski, K., & Spodick, N. (1985). The influence of voice on satisfaction with leaders: exploring the meaning of process control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 7281. 10.1037/0022-3514.48.1.72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, T. (1987). Conditions leading to value expressive effects in judgments of procedural justice: a test of four models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 333344. 10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, T., & Blader, S. (2003). Procedural Justice, social identity, and cooperative behaviour. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 349361. 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0704_07CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Bos, K. (1999). What are we talking about when we talk about no-voice procedures? On the psychology of the fair outcome effect. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 35, 560577. 10.1006/jesp.1999.1393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidmar, N. (1992). Procedural fairness and alternative dispute resolution. Psychological Science, 3(4), 224228. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00033.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behaviour. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(20), 73987401. 10.1073/pnas.0502399102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Ong et al. supplementary material

Instructions for Proposers
Download Ong et al. supplementary material(File)
File 37.9 KB