Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-hxdxx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T09:04:51.523Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do people care about social context? Framing effects in dictator games

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Anna Dreber
Affiliation:
Stockholm School of Economics, Stockholm, Sweden
Tore Ellingsen*
Affiliation:
Stockholm School of Economics, Stockholm, Sweden
Magnus Johannesson
Affiliation:
Stockholm School of Economics, Stockholm, Sweden
David G. Rand
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA

Abstract

Many previous experiments document that behavior in multi-person settings responds to the name of the game and the labeling of strategies. With a few exceptions, these studies cannot tell whether frames affect preferences or beliefs. In three large experiments, we investigate whether social framing effects are also present in Dictator games. Since only one of the subjects makes a decision, the frame can affect behavior merely through preferences. In all the experiments, we find that behavior is insensitive to social framing. We discuss how to reconcile the absence of social framing effects in Dictator games with the presence of social framing effects in Ultimatum games.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2012 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic Supplementary Material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-012-9341-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Amir, O., Rand, D. G., & Gal, Y. K. (2012). Economic games on the internet: the effect of $1 stakes. PLoS ONE, 7. 10.1371/journal.pone.0031461CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreoni, J. (1995). Warm-glow versus cold-prickle: the effects of positive and negative framing on cooperation in experiments. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110, 121. 10.2307/2118508CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Bernheim, B. D. (2009). Social image and the 50–50 norm: A theoretical and experimental analysis of audience effects. Econometrica, 77, 16071636. 10.3982/ECTA7384Google Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Miller, J. (2002). Giving according to GARP: an experimental test of the consistency of preferences for altruism. Econometrica, 70, 737753. 10.1111/1468-0262.00302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Rao, J. (2011). The power of asking: how communication affects selfishness, empathy, and altruism. Journal of Public Economics, 95, 513520. 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2010.12.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armantier, O., & Treich, N. (2010). Eliciting beliefs: proper scoring rules, incentives, stakes and hedging. Manuscript.Google Scholar
Bacharach, M. Binmore, K., Kirmanand, A., & Tani, P. (1993). Variable universe games. Frontiers of game theory, Cambridge: MIT Press 255276.Google Scholar
Bacharach, M. (1999). Interactive team reasoning: A contribution to the theory of co-operation. Research in Economics, 53, 117147. 10.1006/reec.1999.0188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardsley, N. (2008). Dictator game giving: altruism or artifact. Experimental Economics, 11, 122133. 10.1007/s10683-007-9172-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, G. S. (1974). A theory of social interactions. Journal of Political Economy, 82, 10631093. 10.1086/260265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, G. S., & Stigler, G. J. (1977). De gustibus non est disputandum. American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings, 67, 7690.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C., & Xiao, E. (2009). Do the right thing: but only if others do so. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 22, 191208. 10.1002/bdm.621CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanco, M., Engelmann, D., Koch, A., & Normann, H.-T. (2010). Belief elicitation in experiments: is there a hedging problem?. Experimental Economics, 13, 412438. 10.1007/s10683-010-9249-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnet, I., & Frey, B. (1999). Social distance and other-regarding behavior: comment. American Economic Review, 89, 335340. 10.1257/aer.89.1.335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandts, J., & Schwieren, C. (2009). Frames and economic behavior: an experimental study. Manuscript, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Kramer, R. M. (1986). Choice behavior in social dilemmas: effects of social identity, group size, and decision framing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 543549. 10.1037/0022-3514.50.3.543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broberg, T., Ellingsen, T., & Johannesson, M. (2007). Is generosity involuntary?. Economics Letters, 94, 3237. 10.1016/j.econlet.2006.07.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: experiments in strategic interaction, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cappelen, A., Hole, A. D., Sørensen, E.Ø., & Tungodden, B. (2007). The pluralism of fairness ideals: an experimental approach. American Economic Review, 97, 818827. 10.1257/aer.97.3.818CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G., & Gneezy, U. (2008). What’s in a name? Anonymity and social distance in dictator and ultimatum games. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 68, 2935. 10.1016/j.jebo.2008.03.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherry, T. L., Frykblom, P., & Shogren, J. F. (2002). Hardnose the dictator. American Economic Review, 92, 12181221. 10.1257/00028280260344740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cookson, R. (2000). Framing effects in public goods experiments. Experimental Economics, 3, 5579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cubitt, R. P., Drouvelis, M., & Gächter, S. (2011). Framing and free riding: emotional responses and punishment in social dilemma games. Experimental Economics, 14, 254272. 10.1007/s10683-010-9266-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dana, J., Cain, D. M., & Dawes, R. M. (2006). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: costly but quiet exit in dictator games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100, 193201. 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, M. (1958). Trust and suspicion. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 265279. 10.1177/002200275800200401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, M. (1960). The effect of motivational disposition on trust and suspicion. Human Relations, 13, 123139. 10.1177/001872676001300202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufwenberg, M., Gächter, S., & Hennig-Schmidt, H. (2011). The framing of games and the psychology of play. Games and Economic Behavior, 73, 459478. 10.1016/j.geb.2011.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellingsen, T., & Johannesson, M. (2008). Anticipated verbal feedback induces altruistic behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 100105. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M., Mollerstrom, J., & Munkhammar, S. (2012). Social framing effects: preferences or beliefs?. Games and Economic Behavior, 76, 117130. 10.1016/j.geb.2012.05.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M., Torsvik, G., & Tjøtta, S. (2010). Testing guilt aversion. Games and Economic Behavior, 68, 95107. 10.1016/j.geb.2009.04.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, C. (2011). Dictator games: A meta study. Experimental Economics, 14, 583610. 10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., Schmidt, K. Kolm, S.-C., & Ythier, J. M. (2006). The economics of fairness, reciprocity and altruism—experimental evidence and new theories. Handbook of the economics of giving, altruism and reciprocity, Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Gintis, H. (2007). The evolution of private property. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 64, 116. 10.1016/j.jebo.2006.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005). Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 245256. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Handgraaf, M. J. J., van Dijk, E., Vermunt, R. C., Wilke, H. A. M., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2008). Less power or powerless? Egocentric empathy gaps and the irony of having little or no power in social decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 11361149. 10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K. A., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. L. (1994). Preferences, property rights, and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7, 346380. 10.1006/game.1994.1056CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horton, J. J., Rand, D. G., & Zeckhauser, R. J. (2011). The online laboratory: conducting experiments in a real labor market. Experimental Economics, 14, 399425. 10.1007/s10683-011-9273-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houser, D., & Xiao, E. (2009). Avoiding the sharp tongue: anticipated written messages promote fair economic exchange. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 393404. 10.1016/j.joep.2009.05.002Google Scholar
Kay, A. C., & Ross, L. (2003). The perceptual push: the interplay of implicit cues and explicit situational construals on behavioral intentions in the Prisoner’s dilemma. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 634643. 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00057-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keysar, B., Converse, B. A., Wang, J., & Epley, N. (2008). Reciprocity is not give and take: asymmetric reciprocity to positive and negative acts. Psychological Science, 19, 12801286. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02223.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, A., & Norman, H.-T. (2008). Giving in dictator games: regard for others or regard by others?. Southern Economic Journal, 75, 223231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konow, J. (2010). Mixed feelings: theories and evidence on giving. Journal of Public Economics, 94, 279297. 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2009.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kritikos, A., Bolle, F. Agarwal, B., & Vericelli, A. (2005). Utility based altruism: experimental evidence. Psychology, rationality and economic behaviour, New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Krupka, E., & Weber, R. A. (2009). The focusing and informational effects of norms on pro-social behavior. The Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 307320. 10.1016/j.joep.2008.11.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krupka, E., & Weber, R. A. (2010). Identifying social norms using coordination games: why does dictator game sharing vary? Manuscript, Carnegie Mellon University.Google Scholar
Lazear, E. P., Malmendier, U., & Weber, R. A. (2012). Sorting, prices, and social preferences. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 4(1), 136163. 10.1257/app.4.1.136Google Scholar
Leliveld, M. C., van Dijk, E., & van Beest, I. (2008). Ownership in bargaining: introducing the giving, splitting, and taking ultimatum bargaining game. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 12141225. 10.1177/0146167208318600CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149188. 10.1006/obhd.1998.2804CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitt, S., & List, J. (2007). What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world?. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(2), 151174. 10.1257/jep.21.2.153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, V., Samuels, S. M., & Ross, L. (2004). The name of the game: predictive power of reputations versus situational labels in determining prisoner’s dilemma game moves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 11751185. 10.1177/0146167204264004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
List, J. (2007). On the interpretation of giving in dictator games. Journal of Political Economy, 115, 482493. 10.1086/519249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (1999). Experimental economics from the vantage point of behavioural economics. Economic Journal, 109, F25F34. 10.1111/1468-0297.00400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
March, J. (1994). A primer on decision-making: how decisions happen, New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
McCusker, C., & Carnevale, P. J. (1995). Framing in resource dilemmas: loss aversion and the moderating effects on sanctions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 61, 190201. 10.1006/obhd.1995.1015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, W. C., & Sistrunk, F. (1991). Management dilemmas and decisions: impact of framing and anticipated responses. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 35, 2142. 10.1177/0022002791035001002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitzkewitz, M., & Nagel, R. (1993). Experimental results on ultimatum games with incomplete information. International Journal of Game Theory, 22, 171198. 10.1007/BF01243649CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mohlin, E., & Johannesson, M. (2008). Communication: content or relationship?. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 65, 409419. 10.1016/j.jebo.2005.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. D. (1998). Toward a role-theoretic conception of embeddedness. American Journal of Sociology, 104, 92125. 10.1086/210003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 411419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pillutla, M. M., & Chen, X. (1999). Social norms and cooperation in social dilemmas: the effects of context and feedback. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78(2), 81103. 10.1006/obhd.1999.2825CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabin, M. (1998). Psychology and economics. Journal of Economic Literature, 36, 1146.Google Scholar
Rand, D. G. (2012). The promise of Mechanical Turk: how online labor markets can help theorists run behavioral experiments. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 299, 172179. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rege, M., & Telle, K. (2004). The impact of social approval and framing on cooperation in public good situations. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 16251644. 10.1016/S0047-2727(03)00021-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rigdon, M., Ishii, K., Watabe, M., & Kitayama, S. (2009). Minimal social cues in the dictator game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 358367. 10.1016/j.joep.2009.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salant, Y., & Rubinstein, A. (2008). (A, f): choice with frames. Review of Economic Studies, 75, 12871296. 10.1111/j.1467-937X.2008.00510.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sell, J., & Son, Y. (1997). Comparing public goods and common pool resources: three experiments. Social Psychology Quarterly, 60, 118137. 10.2307/2787100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sobel, J. (2005). Interdependent preferences and reciprocity. Journal of Economic Literature, 43, 392436. 10.1257/0022051054661530CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonnemans, J., Schram, A., & Offerman, T. (1998). Public good provision and public bad prevention: the effect of framing. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 34, 143161. 10.1016/S0167-2681(97)00042-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suleiman, R. (1996). Expectations and fairness in a modified ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 17, 531554. 10.1016/S0167-4870(96)00029-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stake, J. E. (2004). The property ‘instinct’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 359, 17631774. 10.1098/rstb.2004.1551Google ScholarPubMed
Sugden, R. (1993). Thinking as a team: towards an explanation of nonselfish behavior. Social Philosophy and Policy, 10, 6989. 10.1017/S0265052500004027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suri, S., & Watts, D. J. (2011). Cooperation and contagion in networked public goods experiments. PLoS ONE, 6. 10.1371/journal.pone.0016836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suvoy, R. (2003). The effects of give and take framing in a dictator game. Unpublished Honors Thesis, University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Tadelis, S. (2008). The power of shame and the rationality of trust. Manuscript, University of California, Berkeley, Haas School of Business.Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453458. 10.1126/science.7455683CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Dijk, E., & Wilke, H. (2000). Decision-induced focusing in social dilemmas: Give-some, keep-some, take-some, and leave-some dilemmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 92104. 10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, J. M. et al., (2004). A conceptual review of decision making in social dilemmas: applying a logic of appropriateness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 281307. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. PNAS, 102, 73987401. 10.1073/pnas.0502399102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhong, C.-B., Loewenstein, J., & Murnighan, J. K. (2007). Speaking the same language: the cooperative effects of μ in the prisoners’ dilemma. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 51, 431456. 10.1177/0022002707300834CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Dreber et al. supplementary material

Dreber et. al. supplementary material
Download Dreber et al. supplementary material(File)
File 254.1 KB