Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-7g5wt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-16T08:29:37.968Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Decomposing desert and tangibility effects in a charitable giving experiment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

David Reinstein
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester, CO4 3SQ, Essex, UK
Gerhard Riener*
Affiliation:
University of Jena, Jena, Germany

Abstract

Several papers have documented that when subjects play with standard laboratory “endowments” they make less self-interested choices than when they use money they have either earned through a laboratory task or brought from outside the lab. In the context of a charitable giving experiment we decompose this into two common artifacts of the laboratory: the intangibility of money (or experimental currency units) promised on a computer screen relative to cash in hand, and the distinct treatment of random “windfall” gains relative to earned money. While both effects are found to be significant in non-parametric tests, the former effect, which has been neglected in previous studies, has a stronger impact on total donations, while the latter effect has a greater impact on the probability of donating. These results have clear implications for experimental design, and also suggest that the availability of more abstract payment methods may increase other-regarding behavior in the field.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2011 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9298-0) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Arabmazar, A., & Schmidt, P. (1981). Further evidence on the robustness of the Tobit estimator to heteroskedasticity. Journal of Econometrics, 17(2), 253258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breman, A. (2006). Give more tomorrow: evidence from a randomized field experiment. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Carlsson, F., He, H., & Martinsson, P. (2009). Easy come, easy go—the role of windfall money in lab and field experiments (Working Papers in Economics 374).Google Scholar
Cherry, T., & Shogren, J. (2008). Self-interest, sympathy and the origin of endowments. Economics Letters, 101(1), 6972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherry, T., Frykblom, P., & Shogren, J. (2002). Hardnose the dictator. American Economic Review, 92(4), 12181221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherry, T., Kroll, S., & Shogren, J. (2005). The impact of endowment heterogeneity and origin on public good contributions: evidence from the lab. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 57(3), 357365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, J. (2002). House money effects in public good experiments. Experimental Economics, 5(3), 223231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cookson, R. (2000). Framing effects in public goods experiments. Experimental Economics, 3(1), 5579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (1996). Altruism in anonymous dictator games. Games and Economic Behavior, 16(2), 181191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (1998). Are women less selfish than men?: evidence from dictator experiments. The Economic Journal, 108(448), 726735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J., Savin, N., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 6(3), 347369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gourieroux, C., Monfort, A., & Trognon, A. (1984). Pseudo maximum likelihood methods: applications to Poisson models. Econometrica, 52(3), 701720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, G. (2007). House money effects in public good experiments: Comment. Experimental Economics, 10(4), 429437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E., & Spitzer, M. (1985). Entitlements, rights, and fairness. Journal of Legal Studies, 14(2), 259297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights, and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7(3), 346380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. (1996). Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games. American Economic Review, 86, 653660.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1991). Anomalies: the endowment effect, loss aversion, and status quo bias. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5(1), 193206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, S., Cherry, T., & Shogren, J. (2007). The impact of endowment heterogeneity and origin on contributions in best-shot public good games. Experimental Economics, 10(4), 411428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
List, J. (2004). Young, selfish and male: field evidence of social preferences. The Economic Journal, 114(492), 121149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. (1988). Two treatises of government. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loomes, G., & Burrows, P. (1994). The impact of fairness on bargaining behaviour. Empirical Economics, 19, 201221.Google Scholar
Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: a theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mittone, L., & Ploner, M. (2006). Is it just legitimacy of endowments? an experimental analysis of unilateral giving (CEEL Working Paper N. 02/2006).Google Scholar
Niederle, M., & Vesterlund, L. (2007). Do women shy away from competition? Do men compete too much? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122(3), 10671101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberholzer-Gee, F., & Eichenberger, R. (1999). Focus effects in dictator game experiments.Google Scholar
Oberholzer-Gee, F., & Eichenberger, R. (2004). Fairness in extended dictator game experiments. Manuscript.Google Scholar
Oxoby, R., & Spraggon, J. (2008). Mine and yours: property rights in dictator games. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organisation, 65, 703713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papke, L., & Wooldridge, J. (1996). Econometric methods for fractional response variables with an application to 401 (k) plan participation rates. Journal of Applied Econometrics, 11(6), 619632.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabin, M. (1993). Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics. American Economic Review, 83(5), 12811302.Google Scholar
Ruffle, B. (1998). More is better, but fair is fair: tipping in dictator and ultimatum games. Games and Economic Behavior, 23(2), 247265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutstrom, E., & Williams, M. (2000). Entitlements and fairness: an experimental study of distributive preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 43(1), 7589.Google Scholar
Sheffrin, H., & Thaler, R. (1988). The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis. Economic Inquiry, 26, 609643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thaler, R. (1985). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 4, 199214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thaler, R., & Johnson, E. (1990). Gambling with the house money and trying to break even: the effects of prior outcomes on risky choice. Management Science, 36, 643660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vohs, K., Mead, N., & Goode, M. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314, 11541156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Reinstein and Riener supplementary material

Reinstein and Riener supplementary material
Download Reinstein and Riener supplementary material(File)
File 139.1 KB