Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-9klzr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T22:19:53.869Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The benefit of the doubt: willful ignorance and altruistic punishment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Robert Stüber*
Affiliation:
WZB, Reichpietschufer 50, 10785 Berlin, Germany

Abstract

Altruistic punishment is often thought to be a major enforcement mechanism of social norms. I present experimental results from a modified version of the dictator game with third-party punishment, in which third parties can remain ignorant about the choice of the dictator. I find that a substantial fraction of subjects choose not to reveal the dictator’s choice and not to punish the dictator. I show that this behavior is in line with the social norms that prevail in a situation of initial ignorance. Remaining ignorant and choosing not to punish is not inappropriate. As a result, altruistic punishment is significantly lower when the dictator’s choice is initially hidden. The decrease in altruistic punishment leads to more selfish dictator behavior only if dictators are explicitly informed about the effect of willful ignorance on punishment rates. Hence, in scenarios in which third parties can ignore information and dictators know what this implies, third-party punishment may only ineffectively enforce social norms.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2019 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-019-09633-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

I am grateful to Philipp Albert, Kai Barron, Björn Bartling, Alexander Cappelen, Marvin Deversi, Urs Fischbacher, Zack Grossman, Rustamdjan Hakimov, Erin Krupka, Dorothea Kübler, Homayoon Moradi, Lisa Spantig, Kristina Strohmaier, Joël van der Weele, Justin Valasek, Roel van Veldhuizen and seminar participants at the ESA World Meeting 2018, the IMEBESS 2018, the SABE/IAREP 2018, the Lisbon Meeting in Economics and Political Science 2018, the theem 2019, and the Berlin Behavioral Economics Workshop for their helpful suggestions. I am also very thankful to Jennifer Rontganger for copy editing.

References

Andreoni, J, Rao, JM, & Trachtman, H (2017). Avoiding the ask: A field experiment on altruism, empathy, and charitable giving. Journal of Political Economy, 125, 625653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aquino, K, & Reed, AI (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balafoutas, L, Grechenig, K, & Nikiforakis, N (2014). Third-party punishment and counter-punishment in one-shot interactions. Economics Letters, 122, 308310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balafoutas, L, & Nikiforakis, N (2012). Norm enforcement in the city: A natural field experiment. European Economic Review, 56, 17731785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balafoutas, L, Nikiforakis, N, & Rockenbach, B (2016). Altruistic punishment does not increase with the severity of norm violations in the field. Nature Communications, 7, 13327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartling, B, Engl, F, & Weber, RA (2014). Does willful ignorance deflect punishment? An experimental study. European Economic Review, 70, 512524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartling, B, & Fischbacher, U (2012). Shifting the blame: On delegation and responsibility. The Review of Economic Studies, 79, 6787.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartling, B, Weber, RA, & Yao, L (2015). Do markets erode social responsibility?. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 130, 219266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, D., & Dana, J. (2012). Paying people to look at the consequences of their actions. Working paper.Google Scholar
Cappelen, A. W., Cappelen, C., & Tungodden, B. (2018). Second-best fairness under limited information: The trade-off between false positives and false negatives. NHH Department of Economics Discussion Paper No. 18/2018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, JP (2007). The demand for punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 62, 522542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, JP, & Matthews, PH (2012). Norm enforcement: Anger, indignation, or reciprocity?. Journal of the European Economic Association, 10, 555572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G, Cobo-Reyes, R, & Jiménez, N (2008). An investment game with third-party intervention. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 68, 1828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coffman, LC (2011). Intermediation reduces punishment (and reward). American Economic Journal: Microeconomics, 3, 77106.Google Scholar
Costa-Gomes, MA, & Weizsäcker, G (2008). Stated beliefs and play in normal-form games. The Review of Economic Studies, 75, 729762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, JC, Servátka, M, & Vadovič, R (2017). Status quo effects in fairness games: Reciprocal responses to acts of commission versus acts of omission. Experimental Economics, 20, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dana, J, Cain, DM, & Dawes, RM (2006). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Costly (but quiet) exit in dictator games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100, 193201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dana, J, Weber, RA, & Kuang, JX (2007). Exploiting moral wiggle room: Experiments demonstrating an illusory preference for fairness. Economic Theory, 33, 6780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DellaVigna, S, List, JA, & Malmendier, U (2012). Testing for altruism and social pressure in charitable giving. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 127, 156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egas, M, & Riedl, A (2008). The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275, 871878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E, & Fischbacher, U (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 6387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E, & Gächter, S (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90, 980994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E, & Schmidt, KM (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114, 817868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E, & Schurtenberger, I (2018). Normative foundations of human cooperation. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 458468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feiler, L (2014). Testing models of information avoidance with binary choice dictator games. Journal of Economic Psychology, 45, 253267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felgendreher, S. (2018). Do consumers choose to stay ignorant? The role of information in the purchase of ethically certified products. Working paper in economics 717, Department of Economics, University of Gothenburg.Google Scholar
Fischbacher, U (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10, 171178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gächter, S, Gerhards, L, & Nosenzo, D (2017). The importance of peers for compliance with norms of fair sharing. European Economic Review, 97, 7286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gächter, S, Nosenzo, D, & Sefton, M (2013). Peer effects in pro-social behavior: Social norms or social preferences?. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11, 548573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gächter, S, & Renner, E (2010). The effects of (incentivized) belief elicitation in public goods experiments. Experimental Economics, 13, 364377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goeschl, T, & Jarke, J (2016). Second and third party punishment under costly monitoring. Journal of Economic Psychology, 54, 124133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greiner, B (2015). Subject pool recruitment procedures: Organizing experiments with ORSEE. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 1, 114125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, Z (2014). Strategic ignorance and the robustness of social preferences. Management Science, 60, 26592665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, Z, & van der Weele, JJ (2017). Self-image and willful ignorance in social decisions. Journal of the European Economic Association, 15, 173217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J, McElreath, R, Barr, A, Ensminger, J, Barrett, C, Bolyanatz, A, Cardenas, JC, Gurven, M, Gwako, E, Henrich, N, Lesorogol, C, Marlowe, F, Tracer, D, & Ziker, J (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312, 17671770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kajackaite, A (2015). If I close my eyes, nobody will get hurt: The effect of ignorance on performance in a real-effort experiment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 116, 518524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandul, S (2016). Ex-post blindness as excuse? The effect of information disclosure on giving. Journal of Economic Psychology, 52, 91101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovářík, J. (2007). Belief formation and evolution in public good games. LABSI Working Paper.Google Scholar
Kriss, PH, Weber, RA, & Xiao, E (2016). Turning a blind eye, but not the other cheek: On the robustness of costly punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 128, 159177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krupka, EL, & Weber, RA (2013). Identifying social norms using coordination games: Why does dictator game sharing vary?. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11, 495524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, T, & Capra, CM (2009). Exploiting moral wiggle room: Illusory preference for fairness? A comment. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazear, EP, Malmendier, U, & Weber, RA (2012). Sorting in experiments with application to social preferences. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 4, 136–63.Google Scholar
Leibbrandt, A, & López-Pérez, R (2012). An exploration of third and second party punishment in ten simple games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 84, 753766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, DK (1998). Modeling altruism and spitefulness in experiments. Review of Economic Dynamics, 1, 593622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewisch, P, Ottone, S, & Ponzano, F (2011). Free-Riding on altruistic punishment? An experimental comparison of third-party punishment in a stand-alone and in an in-group environment. Review of Law & Economics, 7, 161190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
List, JA (2007). On the interpretation of giving in dictator games. Journal of Political economy, 115, 482493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lotz, S, Okimoto, TG, Schlösser, T, & Fetchenhauer, D (2011). Punitive versus compensatory reactions to injustice: Emotional antecedents to third-party interventions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 477480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthey, A., & Regner, T. (2015). Do reciprocators exploit or resist moral wiggle room? An experimental analysis. Jena Economic Research Papers, 9.Google Scholar
Moradi, H., & Nesterov, A. (2017). Moral wiggle room reverted: Information avoidance is myopic. Working paper.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, RO, Ackermann, KA, & Handgraaf, MJ (2011). Measuring social value orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 771781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nikiforakis, N (2008). Punishment and counter-punishment in public good games: Can we really govern ourselves?. Journal of Public Economics, 92, 91112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nikiforakis, N, & Engelmann, D (2011). Altruistic punishment and the threat of feuds. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 78, 319332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nikiforakis, N, & Mitchell, H (2014). Mixing the carrots with the sticks: Third party punishment and reward. Experimental Economics, 17, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyarko, Y, & Schotter, A (2002). An experimental study of belief learning using elicited beliefs. Econometrica, 70, 9711005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oexl, R, & Grossman, Z (2013). Shifting the blame to a powerless intermediary. Experimental Economics, 16, 306312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Regner, T (2018). Reciprocity under moral wiggle room: Is it a preference or a constraint?. Experimental Economics, 21, 779792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trachtman, H, Steinkruger, A, Wood, M, Wooster, A, Andreoni, J, Murphy, JJ, & Rao, JM (2015). Fair weather avoidance: Unpacking the costs and benefits of “avoiding the ask”. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 1, 814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Weele, J. J. (2014). Inconvenient truths: Determinants of strategic ignorance in moral dilemmas. SSRN Working Paper.Google Scholar
van der Weele, JJ, Kulisa, J, Kosfeld, M, & Friebel, G (2014). Resisting moral wiggle room: How robust is reciprocal behavior?. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics, 6, 256264.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Stüber supplementary material

Online Appendix
Download Stüber supplementary material(File)
File 365.8 KB