Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-7g5wt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-16T13:13:16.363Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

State lottery in the lab: an experiment in external validity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Raman Kachurka
Affiliation:
Faculty of Economic Sciences, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
Michał Krawczyk*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Economic Sciences, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
Joanna Rachubik
Affiliation:
Faculty of Economic Sciences, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland

Abstract

In this study, we conduct a laboratory experiment in which the subjects make choices between real-world lottery tickets typically purchased by lottery customers. In this way, we can reliably offer extremely high potential payoffs, something rarely possible in economic experiments. In a between-subject design, we separately manipulate several features that distinguish the situation faced by the customers in the field and by subjects in typical laboratory experiments. We also have the unique opportunity to compare our data to actual sales data provided by the operator of the lottery. We find the distributions to be highly similar (meaning high external validity for this particular setting). The only manipulation that makes a major difference is that when the probabilities of winning specific amounts are explicitly provided (which is not the case in the field), choices shift towards options with lower maximum possible payoff and lower payoff variance. We also find that subjects generally show preference for long shots and that standard laboratory measures of risk posture fail to explain their behavior in the main task.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2021 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-020-09696-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Abt, V, McGurrin, MC, & Smith, JF (1985). Toward a synoptic model of gambling behavior. Journal of Gambling, 1, 7988. 10.1007/BF01019860Google Scholar
Alm, J, Bloomquist, KM, & McKee, M (2015). On the external validity of laboratory tax compliance experiments. Economic Inquiry, 53, 11701186. 10.1111/ecin.12196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, S, Ertaç, S, Gneezy, U, Hoffman, M, & List, JA (2011). Stakes matter in ultimatum games. American Economic Review, 101, 34273439. 10.1257/aer.101.7.3427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariyabuddhiphongs, V, & Chanchalermporn, N (2007). A test of social cognitive theory reciprocal and sequential effects: Hope, superstitious belief and environmental factors among lottery gamblers in Thailand. Journal of Gambling Studies, 23, 201214. 10.1007/s10899-006-9035-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armantier, O, & Treich, N (2015). The Rich Domain Of Risk. Management Science, 10.1287/mnsc.2015.2215Google Scholar
Aydogan, I, & Gao, Y (2020). Experience and rationality under risk: Re-examining the impact of sampling experience. Exp Econ, 10.1007/s10683-019-09641-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barseghyan, L, Prince, J, & Teitelbaum, JC (2011). Are risk preferences stable across contexts? Evidence from insurance data. American Economic Review, 101, 591631. 10.1257/aer.101.2.591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beyerlein, K, & Sallaz, JJ (2017). Faith’s wager: How religion deters gambling. Social Science Research, 62, 204218. 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.07.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohm, P, & Lind, H (1993). Preference reversal, real-world lotteries, and lottery-interested subjects. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 22, 327348. 10.1016/0167-2681(93)90005-ACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chark, R, Chew, SH, & Zhong, S (2020). Individual preference for longshots. Journal of the European Economic Association, 18, 10091039. 10.1093/jeea/jvz004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G., Garcia, T., Offerman, T., & Villeval, M. C. (2019). Do measures of risk attitude in the laboratory predict behavior under risk in and outside of the laboratory? GATE WP 1921. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3398841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G, Gneezy, U, & Imas, A (2013). Experimental methods: Eliciting risk preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 87, 4351. 10.1016/j.jebo.2012.12.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleave, BL, Nikiforakis, N, & Slonim, R (2013). Is there selection bias in laboratory experiments? The case of social and risk preferences. Experimental Economics, 16, 372382. 10.1007/s10683-012-9342-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coups, E, Haddock, G, & Webley, P (1998). Correlates and predictors of lottery play in the United Kingdom. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14, 285303. 10.1023/A:1022009726406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dai, Z, Galeotti, F, & Villeval, MC (2017). Cheating in the lab predicts fraud in the field: An experiment in public transportation. Management Science, 64, 10811100. 10.1287/mnsc.2016.2616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T, Falk, A, Huffman, D, Sunde, U, Schupp, J, & Wagner, GG (2011). Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 9, 522550. 10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorsey, R, & Razzolini, L (2003). Explaining overbidding in first price auctions using controlled lotteries. Experimental Economics, 6, 123140. 10.1023/A:1025375803912CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einav, L, Finkelstein, A, Pascu, I, & Cullen, MR (2012). How general are risk preferences? Choices under uncertainty in different domains. American Economic Review, 102, 26062638. 10.1257/aer.102.6.2606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Exadaktylos, F, Espín, AM, & Brañas-Garza, P (2013). Experimental subjects are not different. Sci Rep, 3, 16. 10.1038/srep01213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, A, Meier, S, & Zehnder, C (2013). Do lab experiments misrepresent social preferences? The case of self-selected student samples. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11, 839852. 10.1111/jeea.12019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr-Duda, H, Bruhin, A, Epper, T, & Schubert, R (2010). Rationality on the rise: Why relative risk aversion increases with stake size. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 40, 147180. 10.1007/s11166-010-9090-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galizzi, M. M., Machado, S. R., & Miniaci, R. (2016). Temporal stability, cross-validity, and external validity of risk preferences measures: Experimental evidence from a UK representative sample. (p. 127). https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2822613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galizzi, MM, & Navarro-Martinez, D (2019). On the external validity of social preference games: a systematic lab-field study. Management Science, 65, 9761002. 10.1287/mnsc.2017.2908CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, B (2002). The relationship between lottery ticket and scratch-card buying behaviour, personality and other compulsive behaviours. Journal of Consumer Behaviour: An International Research Review, 2, 722. 10.1002/cb.86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golec, J, & Tamarkin, M (1998). Bettors love skewness, not risk, at the horse track. Journal of Political Economy, 106, 205225. 10.1086/250007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosling, SD, Rentfrow, PJ, & Swann, WB Jr (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504528. 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, M, & Wood, R (2001). The psychology of lottery gambling. International Gambling Studies, 1, 2745. 10.1080/14459800108732286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, PJ, & Eckel, CC (2015). Loving the long shot: Risk taking with skewed lotteries. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 51, 195217. 10.1007/s11166-015-9228-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, GW, & List, JA (2004). Field experiments. Journal of Economic Literature, 42, 10091055. 10.1257/0022051043004577CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertwig, R, Barron, G, Weber, EU, & Erev, I (2004). Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychological Science, 15, 534539. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00715.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt, CA, & Laury, SK (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92, 16441655. 10.1257/000282802762024700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, J. B., & Vesterlund, L. (2015). The external validity of laboratory experiments: Qualitative rather than quantitative effects. In Handbook of experimental economic methodology. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195328325.003.0020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kontek, K, & Lewandowski, M (2018). Range-dependent utility. Management Science, 64, 28122832. 10.1287/mnsc.2017.2744CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). An application of hierarchical kappa-type statistics in the assessment of majority agreement among multiple observers. Biometrics, 363–374. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitt, S. D. & List, J. A. (2006). What do laboratory experiments tell us about the real world. Presented at the. Journal of Economic Perspectives, Citeseer.Google Scholar
List, J. A. (2020). Non est Disputandum de Generalizability? A Glimpse into The External Validity Trial (Working Paper No. 27535), Working Paper Series. National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w27535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mata, R, Frey, R, Richter, D, Schupp, J, & Hertwig, R (2018). Risk preference: A view from psychology. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32, 155172. 10.1257/jep.32.2.155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, SR, & Zuckerman, M (2003). The relationship of impulsive sensation seeking and gender to interest and participation in gambling activities. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 13851400. 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00357-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, JJ, & Stevens, TH (2004). Contingent valuation, hypothetical bias, and experimental economics. Agricultural and Resource Economics Review, 33, 182192. 10.1017/S1068280500005761CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, P (1998). The cognitive psychology of lottery gambling: A theoretical review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14, 111134. 10.1023/A:1023042708217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, P, & Webley, P (2001). “It Could Be Us!”: Cognitive and social psychological factors in UK national lottery play. Applied Psychology, 50, 181199. 10.1111/1464-0597.00053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trautmann, ST, & Van De Kuilen, G (2015). Ambiguity attitudes. The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making, 1, 89116. 10.1002/9781118468333.ch3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A, & Kahneman, D (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and uncertainty, 5, 297323. 10.1007/BF00122574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollmer, E, Hermann, D, & Mußhoff, O (2017). Is the risk attitude measured with the Holt and Laury task reflected in farmers’ production risk?. European Review of Agricultural Economics, 44, 399424. 10.1093/erae/jbx004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material 1
Download Kachurka et. al. supplementary material(File)
File 115.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material 2
Download Kachurka et. al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.1 MB
Supplementary material: File

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material 3
Download Kachurka et. al. supplementary material(File)
File 3.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material 4
Download Kachurka et. al. supplementary material(File)
File 45.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material 5
Download Kachurka et. al. supplementary material(File)
File 26.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material

Kachurka et. al. supplementary material 6
Download Kachurka et. al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.9 MB