Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-wdhn8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T17:03:07.108Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Circadian effects on strategic reasoning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

David L. Dickinson*
Affiliation:
Dept. of Economics, Appalachian State University, Boone, NC 28608, USA
Todd McElroy
Affiliation:
Dept. of Psychology, Appalachian State University, Boone, NC 28608, USA

Abstract

The ability to strategically reason is important in many competitive environments. In this paper, we examine how relatively mild temporal variations in cognition affect reasoning in the Beauty Contest. The source of temporal cognition variation that we explore is the time-of-day that decisions are made. Our first result is that circadian mismatched subjects (i.e., those making decisions at off-peak time of day) display lower levels of strategic reasoning in the p < 1 Beauty Contest but not in the p > 1 game. This suggests that a cognitively more challenging environment is required for circadian mismatch to harm strategic reasoning. A second result is that choice adaptation or mimicry (i.e., a more automatic type of responding than what is typically considered to be “learning”) during repeated play is not significantly affected by circadian mismatch. This is consistent with the hypothesis that automatic thought is more resilient to cognitive resource depletion than controlled-thought decision making.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2011 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic Supplementary Material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9307-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Adan, A., & Almiral, H. (1991). Horne and Ostberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire: a reduced scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 241253. 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-WCrossRefGoogle Scholar
ǺkerStedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. International Journal of Neuroscience, 52, 2937. 10.3109/00207459008994241CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. C. Jr., & Daugherty, S. R. (2004). Sleep deprivation and fatigue in residency training: results of a national survey of first- and second-year residents. Sleep, 27, 217223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belenky, G., Wesensten, N. J., Thorn, D. R., Thomas, M. L., Sing, H. C. et al., (2003). Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. Journal of Sleep Research, 12, 112. 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00337.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjerner, B., Holm, A., & Swensson, A. (1955). Diurnal variation of mental performance: a study of three-shift workers. British Journal of Industrial Medicine, 12, 103111.Google ScholarPubMed
Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychological Science, 1, 319322. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00226.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch-Domenech, A., Montalvo, J. G., Nagel, R., & Satorra, A. (2002). One, two, (three), infinity, …: newspaper and lab beauty-contest games. American Economic Review, 92, 16871701. 10.1257/000282802762024737CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burks, S. V., Carpenter, J. P., Goette, L., & Rustichini, A. (2009). Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 77457750. 10.1073/pnas.0812360106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116131. 10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2005). Neuroeconomics: how neuroscience can inform economics. Journal of Economic Literature, 43, 964. 10.1257/0022051053737843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chelminski, I., Petros, T. V., Plaud, J. J., & Ferraro, F. R. (2000). Psychometric properties of the reduced Horne and Ostberg questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 469478. 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00208-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coren, S. (1996). Sleep thieves, New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Coren, S. (1996). Daylight savings time and traffic accidents. The New England Journal of Medicine, 334, 924 10.1056/NEJM199604043341416CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coricelli, G., & Nagel, R. (2009). Neural correlates of depth of strategic reasoning in medial prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 91639168. 10.1073/pnas.0807721106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa-Gomes, M., & Crawford, V. P. (2006). Cognition and behavior in two-person guessing games: an experimental study. American Economic Review, 96, 17371768. 10.1257/aer.96.5.1737CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, J., & Nagel, R. (1997). On the robustness of behavior in d experimental beauty-contest games. Econometrics Journal, 107, 16841700.Google Scholar
Ferreira, M. B., Garcia-Marques, L., Sherman, S. J., & Sherman, J. W. (2006). Automatic and controlled components of judgment and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 797813. 10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.797CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosskopf, B., & Nagel, R. (2008). The two person beauty contest. Games and Economic Behavior, 62, 9399. 10.1016/j.geb.2007.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampton, A. N., Bossaerts, P., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2008). Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 67416746. 10.1073/pnas.0711099105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T.-H., Camerer, C., & Weigelt, K. (1998). Iterated dominance and iterated best response in experimental ‘p-beauty contests’. American Economic Review, 88, 947969.Google Scholar
Horne, J. A. (1993). Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour. Implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric behaviour. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 413419. 10.1192/bjp.162.3.413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horne, J. A., & Östberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97110.Google ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, T. S., Cade, B. E., Wolfe, J. M., & Czeisler, C. A. (2003). Searching Night and day: a disassociation of effects of circadian phase and time awake on visual selective attention and vigilance. Psychological Science, 14, 549557. 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1464.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1987). Unconscious influences of memory for a prior event. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 314336. 10.1177/0146167287133003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., Frederick, S. Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (2002). Representativeness revisited: attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. Heuristics & biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment, New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Beatty, J., & Pollack, I. (1967). Perceptual deficit during a mental task. Science, 157, 218219. 10.1126/science.157.3785.218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keynes, J. M. (1936). The general theory of interest, employment and money, London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W., & Pierro, A. (2008). Night and day, you are the one. On circadian mismatches and the transference effect in social perception. Psychological Science, 19, 296301. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02083.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushida, C. A., Chang, A., Gadkary, C., Guilleminault, C., Carrillo, O. et al., (2001). Comparison of actigraphic, polysomnographic, and subjective assessment of sleep parameters in sleep-disordered patients. Sleep Medicine, 2, 389396. 10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00098-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you: nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social exclusion. Psychological Science, 19, 816822. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02162.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMenamin, T. M. (2007). A time to work: recent trends in shift work and flexible schedules. Monthly Labor Review, Dec, 315.Google Scholar
Nagel, R. (1995). Unraveling in guessing games: an experimental study. American Economic Review, 85, 13131326.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. Solso, R. L. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. Information processing and cognition: the Loyola symposium, Hillsdale: Erlbaum 5585.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 166. 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127190. 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 199, 322. 10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. S., Folkard, S., Schmieder, R. A., Parra, L. F., Spelten, E. et al., (2002). Investigation of morning-evening orientation in six countries using the preferences scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 949968. 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00098-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, D. O. (1996). Boundedly rational rule learning in a guessing game. Games and Economic Behavior, 16, 303330. 10.1006/game.1996.0088CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 127, 161188. 10.1037/0096-3445.127.2.161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Dongen, H. P. A., Maislin, G., Mullington, J. M., & Dinges, D. F. (2003). The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep, 26, 117126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, R. A. (2003). Learning with no feedback in a competitive guessing game. Games and Economic Behavior, 44, 134144. 10.1016/S0899-8256(03)00002-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinger, M. B., & Ancoli-Israel, S. (2002). Sleep deprivation and clinical performance. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, 955957. 10.1001/jama.287.8.955CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. J. Pers Soc Psych, 60, 181192. 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, K. P. Jr., Hull, J. T., & Czeisler, C. A. (2002). Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 283, R1370R1377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Dickinson and McElroy supplementary material

Instructions
Download Dickinson and McElroy supplementary material(File)
File 32.1 KB