Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-pdxrj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-11T17:29:56.031Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sleeping and thinking: An examination of insomnia symptoms, global sleep health and sleep regularity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 February 2025

A response to the following question: What role do sleep and circadian rhythms play in psychological functioning including motivation, emotion, cognition, and performance?

Spencer Nielson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Natalie Dautovich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Joseph Dzierzewski*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA National Sleep Foundation, Washington DC, USA
*
Corresponding author: Joseph Dzierzewski; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Introduction:

Subjective cognition is a predictor of cognitive decline and previous work has identified age, education and depression as predictors of subjective cognition. This study aimed to investigate whether several sleep characteristics were associated with subjective cognition above-and-beyond known predictors.

Methods:

Participants (N=3284, Mage=42.7 years, 48.5% female) completed an online study that included the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4), Insomnia Severity Index (ISI), RU-SATED, Sleep Regularity Questionnaire (SRQ) and the 6-item PROMIS Cognitive Function. A 3-step hierarchical regression model predicted PROMIS Cognition scores, with Step 1 including age and education as predictors, Step 2 including age, education, and PHQ-4 scores, and Step 3 including all previous variables and sleep variables.

Results:

In Step 1 (R2=.03), age and education were significant predictors, while in Step 2 (R2=.36), PHQ-4 and education were significant, and age was no longer significant. In Step 3 (R2=.48), PHQ-4, ISI, RU-SATED, and SRQ scores were significant, while age and education were not significant. All steps accounted for a significant increase in variance (p’s<.001).

Conclusions:

Sleep characteristics were associated with subjective cognition above-and-beyond known predictors of age, education and mood. Further research is needed to investigate whether changes in sleep characteristics are associated with changes in subjective cognition.

Type
Results
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Subjective cognition (AKA metacognition or subjective cognitive functioning) refers to an individual’s awareness of, and thoughts about, their own thoughts and cognitive functioning. Subjective cognition is salient across the lifespan as an individual ages and experiences changes in cognitive capabilities. For example, emerging adults’ subjective cognitive functioning can impact their ability to learn new information and skills, while individuals’ negative beliefs about their cognitive functioning are associated with poorer mood and increased anxiety (Arora et al., Reference Arora, Frantz and Toglia2021; Barahmand et al., Reference Barahmand, Abolghasemi and Jahanmohammadi2008; Sheffler et al., Reference Sheffler, Rodriguez, Cheung and Wu2022). Indeed, accurate assessments of one’s cognitive functioning may play an especially salient role for older adults as declines in subjective cognitive functioning have been associated with increased risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia (Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Xie, Xu, Wang, Lei and Yu2022; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Beaumont, Ferguson, Yadegarfar and Stubbs2014; Sella et al., Reference Sella, Cellini, Miola, Sarlo and Borella2019). Further, a meta-analysis demonstrated that older adults with subjective memory complaints were twice as likely to develop dementia than older adults without subjective memory complaints (Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Beaumont, Ferguson, Yadegarfar and Stubbs2014). Because of its implications for an individual’s health, investigating the predictors of subjective cognitive functioning, and especially predictors of its potential decline in older adulthood, has been a priority of the field.

Previous studies into predictors of subjective cognitive functioning across the lifespan have highlighted demographic factors including age and level of education, as consistent predictors of subjective cognitive functioning, such that older age and lower levels of education are associated with worse subjective cognitive functioning (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015; Dzierzewski, Reference Dzierzewski2022). Moreover, mental health factors, including symptoms of depression, have been shown to be associated with poorer subjective cognitive functioning as well (Doos Ali Vand et al., Reference Doos Ali Vand, Gharraee, Farid and Bandi2014; Molinuevo et al., Reference Molinuevo, Rabin, Amariglio, Buckley, Dubois, Ellis, Ewers, Hampel, Klöppel, Rami, Reisberg, Saykin, Sikkes, Smart, Snitz, Sperling, van der Flier, Wagner and Jessen2017; Zlatar et al., Reference Zlatar, Muniz, Espinoza, Gratianne, Gollan, Galasko and Salmon2018). One study reported that individuals with higher levels of both depression and subjective cognitive decline were at the highest risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia, compared to individuals who had just one or the other (Liew, Reference Liew2019). Thus, age, education and mental health have all been shown to be important in understanding an individual’s subjective cognitive functioning. However, it is currently unknown whether and how sleep, an aspect of health that changes across the lifespan and is highly associated with mental health, is associated with subjective cognitive functioning in light of known predictors.

Sleep is essential to an individual’s health and functioning. Poor sleep has been associated with increased health risks including increased risk of cardiovascular disease, obesity, diabetes and all-cause mortality (Bansil et al., Reference Bansil, Kuklina, Merritt and Yoon2011; Cappuccio et al., Reference Cappuccio, D’Elia, Strazzullo and Miller2010, Reference Cappuccio, Cooper, D’Elia, Strazzullo and Miller2011). Previous work has also demonstrated that sleep is essential for optimal cognitive functioning (Dzierzewski, Reference Dzierzewski2022; Dzierzewski et al., Reference Dzierzewski, Perez, Ravyts and Dautovich2022; Kronholm et al., Reference Kronholm, Sallinen, Suutama, Sulkava, Era and Partonen2009; Wardle-Pinkston et al., Reference Wardle-Pinkston, Slavish and Taylor2019). Specifically, better sleep health, including more regular sleep, has been previously associated with better cognition (Boeve et al., Reference Boeve, Halpin, Michaud, Fagan and MacAulay2022; Gildner Theresa et al., Reference Gildner Theresa, Liebert Melissa, Paul, Somnath and Josh2024; Sletten et al., Reference Sletten, Weaver, Foster, Gozal, Klerman, Rajaratnam, Roenneberg, Takahashi, Turek, Vitiello, Young and Czeisler2023). Moreover, one daytime deficit commonly associated with insomnia symptoms is poor cognitive performance (Wardle-Pinkston et al., Reference Wardle-Pinkston, Slavish and Taylor2019). Although how sleep impacts cognition remains poorly understood, Dzierzewski et al. (Reference Dzierzewski, Perez, Ravyts and Dautovich2022) proposed a model that suggests various biological (e.g., inflammation, metabolic processes), psychological (e.g., depression, anxiety) and social (e.g., loneliness) processes that may underlie the sleep-cognition association, including the association between sleep and subjective cognitive functioning.

An emerging area of research has begun to investigate how sleep is associated with an individual’s subjective cognitive functioning. Previous studies have demonstrated that poor sleep, including symptoms of insomnia, is associated with poorer subjective cognition across the lifespan, including one study which observed that middle-aged and older adults who reported worse sleep 3 years after a baseline survey showed significantly increased odds of reporting declines in their cognitive capacities (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Curtis, Musich, Guandique and McCrae2023; Doos Ali Vand et al., Reference Doos Ali Vand, Gharraee, Farid and Bandi2014; Joo et al., Reference Joo, Joo, Kwon, Jang and Park2021; Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Cross, Yao, Carrier, Postuma, Gosselin, Kakinami and Dang-Vu2022). Moreover, another study observed that the association between subjective cognitive decline and insomnia symptoms was mediated by depression, thereby indicating that there may be underlying factors that affect this association (Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Xie, Xu, Wang, Lei and Yu2022). Other studies have demonstrated that poor sleep quality and short and long sleep durations are associated with increased risk of subjective decline in cognition (Joo et al., Reference Joo, Joo, Kwon, Jang and Park2021; Kang et al., Reference Kang, Yoon, Lee, Kim, Lee, Han, Kim and Kim2017). However, few studies have investigated whether other aspects of sleep health, including global sleep health and sleep regularity, are associated with subjective cognitive functioning despite previous evidence suggesting their association with objective cognition and better health (Boeve et al., Reference Boeve, Halpin, Michaud, Fagan and MacAulay2022; Gildner Theresa et al., Reference Gildner Theresa, Liebert Melissa, Paul, Somnath and Josh2024; Sletten et al., Reference Sletten, Weaver, Foster, Gozal, Klerman, Rajaratnam, Roenneberg, Takahashi, Turek, Vitiello, Young and Czeisler2023). Investigation of sleep characteristics outside of insomnia symptoms, including sleep health and sleep regularity, may further clarify the association between sleep and subjective cognitive functioning. Further, research that investigates how subjective cognitive functioning is associated with sleep characteristics while accounting for previously identified predictors of subjective cognitive functioning, may help differentiate whether these previously identified predictors (e.g., mood) underlie the sleep-subjective cognition association.

This study aimed to investigate whether sleep characteristics, including insomnia symptoms, global sleep health and sleep regularity, were associated with subjective cognitive functioning above-and-beyond known predictors of age, education and mood. Based on previous research, it was hypothesized that fewer insomnia symptoms, better sleep regularity and better global sleep health would be associated with better subjective cognitive functioning.

Methods

Protocol

Data were collected as part of an online survey investigating sleep and health across the lifespan. Participants were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online platform that allows individuals to participate in a variety of research studies for compensation. To be eligible for inclusion, individuals had to be at least 18 years old, be able to respond to surveys in English, and have access to a computer or tablet that they could use to complete the survey. Participants signed up for the study in MTurk, gave electronic informed consent, and completed the survey through Qualtrics. All protocols and procedures for the study were approved by the local Institutional Review Board (#HM20020410) and were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were compensated $0.25 upon completion of the survey. It has been previously shown that compensation amounts do not affect the quality of data received through MTurk (Buhrmester et al., Reference Buhrmester, Kwang and Gosling2011). Although Amazon’s MTurk data collection has been observed to be as reliable as paper and pencil surveys (Buhrmester et al., Reference Buhrmester, Kwang and Gosling2011), two validity checks were incorporated into the survey. First, participants were asked to report their age at the beginning of the survey and then report their birth year at the end of the survey. Secondly, at a specific point in the survey, participants were asked to respond to a question (i.e., “We are interested in whether respondents are reading the instructions for each question. Please choose the once or twice-a-week response option to show you are paying attention”) with a specific response to certify that they were paying attention during the survey. Individuals who did not pass these validity checks were excluded from all analyses.

Measures

Demographics: Demographic questions included age, self-reported sex, level of education, race and ethnicity.

Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer, Williams and Löwe2009): The Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) is a 4-item questionnaire that measures the frequency of symptoms of depression and anxiety over the past two weeks (e.g., “over the past 2 weeks, how often have you been bothered by the following problems… Little interest or pleasure in doing things).” Each item includes a 4-point Likert-type scale with answer choices ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day). The total score ranges from 0-12, with higher scores indicating increased severity. Separate sub-scores for symptoms of depression and anxiety can be calculated, though the total score combining the two subscale scores was utilized in these analyses. The Cronbach’s alpha for the PHQ-4 in the present study was 0.895.

Insomnia Severity Index (Bastien et al., Reference Bastien, Vallières and Morin2001): The Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) is a well-known, widely used self-report questionnaire that evaluates nighttime and daytime symptoms of insomnia over the past two weeks (e.g., “Please rate the severity of your current insomnia problem”, “To what extent do you consider your sleep problem to INTERFERE with your daily functioning”). This 7-item questionnaire is scored on a 5-point Likert Scale and has a range of 0-28, with higher scores indicating more severe insomnia symptoms. A cutoff score of 10 or higher has been noted to be sensitive to detecting insomnia status in community-based samples (Morin et al., Reference Morin, Belleville, Bélanger and Ivers2011). The Cronbach’s alpha for this measure in the present study was 0.884.

RU-SATED sleep health questionnaire (Buysse, Reference Buysse2014; Ravyts et al., Reference Ravyts, Dzierzewski, Perez, Donovan and Dautovich2021): The RU-SATED questionnaire is a 6-item measure that assesses six dimensions of sleep health including regularity, satisfaction, alertness, timing, efficiency and duration. Participants are asked how often they follow the sleep health guidelines regarding these six dimensions (e.g., “Do you spend less than 30 minutes awake at night?”) on a 3-point Likert type scale ranging from 0 (Rarely/Never) to 2 (Usually/Always), with total scores ranging from 0-12. Higher scores indicate better sleep health. One previous study reported a 2-factor structure of the RUSATED scale, though further research is needed to replicate this factor structure (Ravyts et al., Reference Ravyts, Dzierzewski, Perez, Donovan and Dautovich2021). The total score was used for the analyses in the present study. Cronbach’s alpha for this measure in the current sample is 0.637.

Sleep Regularity Questionnaire (Dzierzewski et al., Reference Dzierzewski, Donovan and Sabet2021): The Sleep Regularity Questionnaire (SRQ) was used to assess the degree to which individuals engage in consistent sleep behaviors (e.g., “I go to bed at about the same time every night,” “I wake up at about the same time each morning”). Participants rated each of the 6 items on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much), with scores ranging from 6-30. Higher scores indicate more regularity in sleep. Cronbach’s alpha for the 6-item measure in the current sample was 0.869.

PROMIS Cognitive Function-Short Form (Iverson et al., Reference Iverson, Marsh, Connors and Terry2021; Lai et al., Reference Lai, Wagner, Jacobsen and Cella2014): The PROMIS Cognitive Function Short form is a 6-item measure derived from the greater PROMIS cognitive function measure. Sample items include “My thinking has been slow” and “It has seemed like my brain was not working as well as usual…” Items use a 1-5 scale with options ranging from “very often” to “never”, with scores ranging from 6-30. Higher scores indicate better subjective cognitive functioning. The Cronbach’s alpha for this measure in the present study was 0.958.

Analyses

IBM SPSS Statistics version 29 was used to conduct analyses (IBM Corp., 2023). Hierarchical linear regression, in which predictors are entered into the model in blocks to determine whether subsequent predictors account for a significant change in the variance explained, was used to investigate whether ISI, RU-SATED and SRQ scores predicted PROMIS Cognitive Function scores after accounting for age, education and PHQ-4 scores. PROMIS Cognitive Function scores were the outcome variable in every step. In step 1 of the model, known demographic predictors of age and level of education were entered into the model. In step 2, PHQ-4 total score was added to the model, while age and education were kept in the model. In step 3, all previous variables were entered into the model, followed by the three sleep variables (i.e., ISI, RU-SATED, SRQ). Due to the conceptual overlap between the sleep variables, multicollinearity was inspected based on the variance inflation factor (VIF), with VIFs greater than 10 indicating a bias in the model due to multicollinearity (Field, Reference Field2018).

Results

Participant demographics are reported in Table 1. In brief, participants (N=3284) were generally middle-aged adults (M age=42.7 years), split between males and females (48.5% female), and had a 4-year bachelor’s degree. Participants had sub-clinical levels of depression (M =1.59) and anxiety (M = 1.73) symptoms, with the total score of the PHQ-4 also being in the sub-clinical range (M = 3.32). Regarding sleep, participants generally reported sub-clinical insomnia symptoms (M = 8.49), and moderate levels of sleep health (M = 7.59) and sleep regularity (M = 22.39). The RU-SATED scale has been previously shown to have a two-factor structure, with one factor relating to sleep quality & quantity and the other factor relating to circadian rhythm (Ravyts et al., Reference Ravyts, Dzierzewski, Perez, Donovan and Dautovich2021). In this study, participants reported moderate levels of sleep quality and quantity (M = 3.23), and slightly higher levels of sleep health characteristics related to circadian rhythm (M = 4.35). Subjective cognitive functioning was in the moderate range (M = 22.01).

Table 1. Demographic characteristics (N = 3284)

Note: Participants could select more than one race/ethnicity.

Table 2 reports the results of the hierarchical linear regression models. All VIFs were less than 10, indicating no issues with multicollinearity (Field, Reference Field2018). In Step 1 (R 2 =0.03) of the model, which included only age and level of education as predictor variables, older age and higher education predicted better subjective cognitive functioning (β =.146, t=8.44, p <.001; β =.086, t=4.97, p <.001, respectively). In Step 2 (R 2=.36), PHQ-4 scores and education were significant while age was no longer significant (β= −.589, t= −40.56, p<.001; β =.03, t=2.10, p=.036, respectively), such that better mental health and higher education predicted better subjective cognition. Lastly, in Step 3 (R 2=.48), scores on the PHQ-4, ISI, RUSATED and SRQ were significant predictors of subjective cognitive functioning (β = −.346, t=−21.93, p<.001; β = −.380, t=−20.20, p<.001; β = 0.040, t=2.13, p =.033; β = 0.037, t=2.35, p =.019, respectively), such that better mental health, fewer insomnia symptoms and better sleep health and sleep regularity predicted better subjective cognition, while age and education were not significant predictors. All steps accounted for a significant increase in variance explained (Step 1: ΔR 2=.031, F=53.09, p<.001; Step 2: ΔR 2=.324, F=1644.79, p<.001; Step 3: ΔR 2=.121, F=251.80, p<.001).

Table 2. Hierarchical regression models

Note: Bold indicates significance. The PROMIS Cognitive Functioning Questionnaire was the outcome variable in each model. Each model was significant at p <.001 and accounted for a significant increase in variance.

Conclusions

This study found that sleep characteristics, including insomnia symptoms, global sleep health and sleep regularity, were associated with subjective cognitive functioning above-and-beyond known predictors of age, education and mood. Of note, adding in the sleep variables accounted for an additional 12% of the variance in subjective cognition. Those who reported less insomnia symptoms, greater sleep health and more regular sleep reported higher levels of subjective cognition. This study also observed that older age and higher levels of education were associated with better subjective cognitive functioning, though these variables were not significant when including sleep characteristics, which might suggest that non-demographic factors may have more salience than demographic factors when considering the sleep-subjective cognition association. Future research that replicates and extends these findings could potentially elucidate underlying factors that are implicated in the sleep-subjective cognition association, including the potential role of sleep and mood disorders, and whether changes in sleep health correspond with changes in subjective cognition.

The results of the present study align with previous literature examining the association between sleep and subjective cognitive functioning. Previous work has demonstrated that poor sleep quality is associated with poorer subjective cognition, while sleep duration had a U-shaped association, such that long (>9 hours) and short (<5 hours) sleep durations were associated with poorer subjective cognition (Joo et al., Reference Joo, Joo, Kwon, Jang and Park2021). Further work observed that increased insomnia symptoms are associated with poorer subjective cognition (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Curtis, Musich, Guandique and McCrae2023; Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Cross, Yao, Carrier, Postuma, Gosselin, Kakinami and Dang-Vu2022), which was also observed in the present study. This study extended the current literature by observing that other sleep characteristics beyond insomnia symptoms, including sleep health and sleep regularity, are associated with subjective cognition, thereby demonstrating that sleep characteristics outside of disordered sleep may be important in understanding the sleep-subjective cognition association. Sleep health and sleep regularity have been previously shown to be associated with cognitive performance, and have ties to other health outcomes such as inflammation, metabolic outcomes, cardiovascular disease and mortality (Baron et al., Reference Baron, Reid, Malkani, Kang and Zee2017; Dzierzewski et al., Reference Dzierzewski, Donovan, Kay, Sannes and Bradbrook2020; Grandner et al., Reference Grandner, Chakravorty, Perlis, Oliver and Gurubhagavatula2014; Sletten et al., Reference Sletten, Weaver, Foster, Gozal, Klerman, Rajaratnam, Roenneberg, Takahashi, Turek, Vitiello, Young and Czeisler2023; Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Stone, Smagula, Hall, Simsek, Kado, Redline, Vo and Buysse2018). Importantly, these results also suggest that sleep health across the spectrum, rather than just disordered sleep, is associated with subjective cognition, alluding to the importance of maintaining healthy and regular sleep. Although it is still unknown what mechanisms underlie the sleep-subjective cognition association, the model proposed by Dzierzewski et al. (Reference Dzierzewski, Perez, Ravyts and Dautovich2022) suggests biological (e.g., inflammation), psychological (e.g., depression) and social (e.g., loneliness) mechanisms that may underlie this association and provides potential avenues for future research. For example, this study found that mood (i.e., depression and anxiety) symptoms were associated with subjective cognition, even when accounting for sleep characteristics, which may suggest that mood may be an important factor in the sleep-subjective cognition association. Clinically, this study suggests that sleep characteristics, including insomnia symptoms, global sleep health and sleep regularity, are an important factor to consider in subjective cognition and future studies which investigate how changes in sleep characteristics are associated with changes in subjective cognition could elucidate how clinical interventions in sleep may affect changes in subjective cognition.

Limitations and future directions

Due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, we are unable to examine directionality in the sleep-subjective cognition association. Future research that examines temporal associations between sleep and subjective cognitive functioning may help elucidate if changes in subjective cognitive functioning precede changes in sleep, or vice versa. Moreover, because of the self-report nature of the data, results may have been affected by recall bias, social desirability bias and other biases that commonly affect self-report data, though the anonymous nature of the survey may have helped limit the effect of these biases. Another limitation of the present study is that we did not include other sleep behaviors such as total sleep time, sleep onset latency and wake after sleep onset. While the present study is an important first step in extending our understanding regarding the sleep-subjective cognition association, investigating how these sleep behaviors are associated with one’s subjective cognitive functioning may elucidate whether the association differs based on the actual nighttime sleep of an individual. Moreover, future studies that investigate whether and how other demographic factors (e.g., race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, marital status, employment status) affect the association between sleep and subjective cognition could help elucidate whether this association is modified by social determinants of health. For example, there have been mixed results regarding the association between race/ethnicity and subjective cognition (Katz et al., Reference Katz, Qin, Wang and Rabin2023; Wooten et al., Reference Wooten, McGuire, Olivari, Jackson and Croft2023) and few, if any studies, have investigated how race and ethnicity potentially affect the sleep-subjective cognition association. Such research would help to elucidate whether cultural factors (e.g., discrimination, acculturation, neighborhood noise) affect the sleep-subjective cognition association. A final limitation to note about the present study is that, although the present data were collected from a community-based sample, we are unable to generalize the results of the present sample to other populations due to the convenient nature of the present sample. Future studies that utilize more random samples can provide insight into whether this association is generalizable.

This study observed that people reporting fewer insomnia symptoms, greater sleep health and more regular sleep were more likely to also experience greater subjective cognitive functioning. Future research that investigates how sleep behaviors, including total sleep time and sleep efficiency, are associated with one’s subjective cognitive functioning, and whether changes in sleep are associated with changes in subjective cognition, may help further elucidate underlying factors of the sleep-subjective cognition association.

Data availability statement

The data are not available publicly because they were derived from a larger dataset not yet publicly available. Raw data were generated at Virginia Commonwealth University. Derived data supporting the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author JMD upon reasonable request.

Author’s contributions

Spencer A. Nielson: Conceptualization, Formal analyses, Writing – original draft.

Natalie D. Dautovich: Writing – original draft, Supervision

Joseph Dzierzewski: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analyses, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Supervision, Project administration.

Financial support

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Aging, USA of the National Institutes of Health, USA under Award Number K23AG049955 (PI: Dzierzewski).

Competing interests

Joseph Dzierzewski served on an advisory panel for Eisai Pharmaceuticals and received an honorarium for a presentation given to the Nevada Psychological Association.

All other co-authors have no interests to disclose.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was obtained from the ethics committee of Virginia Commonwealth University (Reference number: #HM20020410) Study participants gave electronic informed consent to take part in the study.

References

Connections references

Kay, D. B., & Dzierzewski, J. M. (2024). What role do sleep and circadian rhythms play in psychological functioning including motivation, emotion, cognition and performance? Research Directions: Sleep Psychology, 1, e4. https://doi.org/10.1017/slp.2023.5.Google Scholar

References

Arora, C, Frantz, C and Toglia, J (2021) Awareness of performance on a functional cognitive performance-based assessment across the adult lifespan. Frontiers in Psychology 12, 753016. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.753016 Google Scholar
Bansil, P, Kuklina, EV, Merritt, RK and Yoon, PW (2011) Associations between sleep disorders, sleep duration, quality of sleep, and hypertension: Results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2005 to 2008. Journal of Clinical Hypertension (Greenwich, Conn.) 13(10), 739743. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-7176.2011.00500.x Google Scholar
Barahmand, U, Abolghasemi, A and Jahanmohammadi, S (2008) Using metacognitions to identify emotionally vulnerable college students. American Journal of Health Behavior 32(6), 604613. https://doi.org/10.5555/ajhb.2008.32.6.604 Google Scholar
Baron, KG, Reid, KJ, Malkani, RG, Kang, J and Zee, PC (2017) Sleep variability among older adults with insomnia: Associations with sleep quality and cardiometabolic disease risk. Behavioral Sleep Medicine 15(2), 144157. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2015.1120200 Google Scholar
Bastien, CH, Vallières, A and Morin, CM (2001) Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine 2(4), 297307. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1389-9457(00)00065-4 Google Scholar
Baumgart, M, Snyder, HM, Carrillo, MC, Fazio, S, Kim, H and Johns, H (2015) Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: A population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association 11(6), 718726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016 Google Scholar
Boeve, A, Halpin, A, Michaud, S, Fagan, M and MacAulay, RK (2022) Specific sleep health domains as predictors of executive function in older adults. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 34(4), 422427. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.21040112 Google Scholar
Buhrmester, M, Kwang, T and Gosling, S. D (2011) Amazon’s mechanical turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 6(1), 35. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980 Google Scholar
Buysse, DJ (2014) Sleep health: Can we define it? does it matter? Sleep 37(1), 917. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.3298 Google Scholar
Cappuccio, FP, Cooper, D, D’Elia, L, Strazzullo, P and Miller, MA (2011) Sleep duration predicts cardiovascular outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. European Heart Journal 32(12), 14841492. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurheartj/ehr007 Google Scholar
Cappuccio, FP, D’Elia, L, Strazzullo, P and Miller, MA (2010) Sleep duration and all-cause mortality: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Sleep 33(5), 585592. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/33.5.585 Google Scholar
Costa, AN, Curtis, AF, Musich, M, Guandique, AA and McCrae, CS (2023) Self-reported cognition in older adults with insomnia: Associations with sleep and domain specific cognition. Journal of Sleep Research 32(1), e13751. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13751 Google Scholar
Doos Ali Vand, H, Gharraee, B, Farid, A-AA and Bandi, MG (2014) Prediction of insomnia severity based on cognitive, metacognitive, and emotional variables in college students. Explore (New York, NY) 10(4), 233240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.explore.2014.04.005 Google Scholar
Dzierzewski, JM (2022) Insomnia and subjective cognitive decline in older adults: Avenues for continued investigation and potential intervention. Sleep 45(11), zsac216. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsac216 Google Scholar
Dzierzewski, JM, Donovan, EK, Kay, DB, Sannes, TS and Bradbrook, KE (2020) Sleep inconsistency and markers of inflammation. Frontiers in Neurology 11, 1042. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2020.01042 Google Scholar
Dzierzewski, JM, Donovan, EK and Sabet, SM (2021) The sleep regularity questionnaire: Development and initial validation. Sleep Medicine 85, 4553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2021.06.028 Google Scholar
Dzierzewski, JM, Perez, E, Ravyts, SG and Dautovich, N (2022) Sleep and cognition: A narrative review focused on older adults. Sleep Medicine Clinics 17(2), 205222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsmc.2022.02.001 Google Scholar
Field, A (2018) Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 5th Edn. SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Gildner Theresa, E, Liebert Melissa, A, Paul, K, Somnath, C and Josh, SJ (2024) Associations between sleep duration, sleep quality, and cognitive test performance among older adults from six middle income countries: Results from the Study on Global Ageing and Adult Health (SAGE). Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine 10(06), 613621. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.3782 Google Scholar
Grandner, MA, Chakravorty, S, Perlis, ML, Oliver, L and Gurubhagavatula, I (2014) Habitual sleep duration associated with self-reported and objectively determined cardiometabolic risk factors. Sleep Medicine 15(1), 4250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2013.09.012 Google Scholar
IBM Corp. (2023) IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 29.0.2.0) [Computer software]. IBM Corp.Google Scholar
Iverson, GL, Marsh, JM, Connors, EJ and Terry, DP (2021) Normative reference values, reliability, and item-level symptom endorsement for the PROMIS® v2.0 cognitive function-short Forms 4a, 6a and 8a. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists 36(7), 13411349. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acaa128 Google Scholar
Jiang, H-X, Xie, X, Xu, Y, Wang, R, Lei, X and Yu, J (2022) Older adults’ subjective cognitive decline correlated with subjective but not objective sleep: A mediator role of depression. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development 95(1), 4256. https://doi.org/10.1177/00914150211024186 Google Scholar
Joo, HJ, Joo, JH, Kwon, J, Jang, BN and Park, E-C (2021) Association between quality and duration of sleep and subjective cognitive decline: A cross-sectional study in South Korea. Scientific Reports 11(1), 16989. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-96453-x Google Scholar
Kang, S-H, Yoon, I-Y, Lee, SD, Kim, T, Lee, CS, Han, JW, Kim, KW and Kim, C-H (2017) Subjective memory complaints in an elderly population with poor sleep quality. Aging & Mental Health 21(5), 532536. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1124839 Google Scholar
Katz, MJ, Qin, J, Wang, C and Rabin, L (2023) Subjective cognitive concerns differ by race/ethnicity among einstein aging study (EAS) participants: Implications for assessment and interpretation of self-perceived cognitive functioning. Alzheimer’s & Dementia 19(S4), e065943. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.065943 Google Scholar
Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW and Löwe, B (2009) An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: The PHQ-4. Psychosomatics 50(6), 613621. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613 Google Scholar
Kronholm, E, Sallinen, M, Suutama, T, Sulkava, R, Era, P and Partonen, T (2009) Self-reported sleep duration and cognitive functioning in the general population. Journal of Sleep Research 18(4), 436446. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00765.x Google Scholar
Lai, J-S, Wagner, LI, Jacobsen, PB and Cella, D (2014) Self-reported cognitive concerns and abilities: Two sides of one coin? Psycho-Oncology 23(10), 11331141. https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.3522 Google Scholar
Liew, TM (2019) Depression, subjective cognitive decline, and the risk of neurocognitive disorders. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy 11(1), 70. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-019-0527-7 Google Scholar
Mitchell, AJ, Beaumont, H, Ferguson, D, Yadegarfar, M and Stubbs, B (2014) Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: Meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 130(6), 439451. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12336 Google Scholar
Molinuevo, JL, Rabin, LA, Amariglio, R, Buckley, R, Dubois, B, Ellis, KA, Ewers, M, Hampel, H, Klöppel, S, Rami, L, Reisberg, B, Saykin, AJ, Sikkes, S, Smart, CM, Snitz, BE, Sperling, R, van der Flier, WM, Wagner, M and Jessen, F (2017) Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association 13(3), 296311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2016.09.012 Google Scholar
Morin, CM, Belleville, G, Bélanger, L and Ivers, H (2011) The Insomnia Severity Index: Psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep 34(5), 601608. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/34.5.601 Google Scholar
Ravyts, SG, Dzierzewski, JM, Perez, E, Donovan, EK and Dautovich, ND (2021) Sleep health as measured by RU SATED: A psychometric evaluation. Behavioral Sleep Medicine 19(1), 4856. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402002.2019.1701474 Google Scholar
Sella, E, Cellini, N, Miola, L, Sarlo, M and Borella, E (2019) The influence of metacognitive beliefs on sleeping difficulties in older adults. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being 11(1), 2041. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12140 Google Scholar
Sheffler, P, Rodriguez, TM, Cheung, CS and Wu, R (2022) Cognitive and metacognitive, motivational, and resource considerations for learning new skills across the lifespan. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science 13(2), e1585. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1585 Google Scholar
Sletten, TL, Weaver, MD, Foster, RG, Gozal, D, Klerman, EB, Rajaratnam, SMW, Roenneberg, T, Takahashi, JS, Turek, FW, Vitiello, MV, Young, MW and Czeisler, CA (2023) The importance of sleep regularity: A consensus statement of the National Sleep Foundation sleep timing and variability panel. Sleep Health: Journal of the National Sleep Foundation 9(6), 801820. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2023.07.016 Google Scholar
Wallace, ML, Stone, K, Smagula, SF, Hall, MH, Simsek, B, Kado, DM, Redline, S, Vo, TN, Buysse, DJ and Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study Research Group. (2018) Which sleep health characteristics predict all-cause mortality in older men? An application of flexible multivariable approaches. Sleep 41(1). https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsx189 Google Scholar
Wardle-Pinkston, S, Slavish, DC and Taylor, DJ (2019) Insomnia and cognitive performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews 48, 101205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2019.07.008 Google Scholar
Wooten, KG, McGuire, LC, Olivari, BS, Jackson, EMJ and Croft, JB (2023) Racial and ethnic differences in subjective cognitive decline—United States, 2015-2020. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 72(10), 249255. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm7210a1 Google Scholar
Zhao, J-L, Cross, N, Yao, CW, Carrier, J, Postuma, RB, Gosselin, N, Kakinami, L and Dang-Vu, TT (2022) Insomnia disorder increases the risk of subjective memory decline in middle-aged and older adults: A longitudinal analysis of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Sleep 45(11), zsac176. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsac176 Google Scholar
Zlatar, ZZ, Muniz, MC, Espinoza, SG, Gratianne, R, Gollan, TH, Galasko, D and Salmon, DP (2018) Subjective cognitive decline, objective cognition, and depression in older hispanics screened for memory impairment. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease: JAD 63(3), 949956. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170865 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics (N = 3284)

Figure 1

Table 2. Hierarchical regression models

Author comment: Sleeping and Thinking: An Examination of Insomnia Symptoms, Global Sleep Health, and Sleep Regularity — R0/PR1

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Decision: Sleeping and Thinking: An Examination of Insomnia Symptoms, Global Sleep Health, and Sleep Regularity — R0/PR2

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Presentation

Overall score 4 out of 5
Is the article written in clear and proper English? (30%)
4 out of 5
Is the data presented in the most useful manner? (40%)
4 out of 5
Does the paper cite relevant and related articles appropriately? (30%)
4 out of 5

Context

Overall score 4 out of 5
Does the title suitably represent the article? (25%)
5 out of 5
Does the abstract correctly embody the content of the article? (25%)
5 out of 5
Does the introduction give appropriate context and indicate the relevance of the results to the question or hypothesis under consideration? (25%)
3 out of 5
Is the objective of the experiment clearly defined? (25%)
4 out of 5

Results

Overall score 4 out of 5
Is sufficient detail provided to allow replication of the study? (50%)
4 out of 5
Are the limitations of the experiment as well as the contributions of the results clearly outlined? (50%)
4 out of 5

Author comment: Sleeping and Thinking: An Examination of Insomnia Symptoms, Global Sleep Health, and Sleep Regularity — R1/PR3

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Decision: Sleeping and Thinking: An Examination of Insomnia Symptoms, Global Sleep Health, and Sleep Regularity — R1/PR4

Comments

No accompanying comment.