Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-g9frx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-17T13:51:52.879Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hunger and the gender gap

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Yan Chen*
Affiliation:
School of Information, University of Michigan, 105 South State Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2112, USA Department of Economics, School of Economics and Management, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084, China
Ming Jiang*
Affiliation:
Antai College of Economics and Management, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, 1954 Huashan Road, Shanghai 200434, China
Erin L. Krupka*
Affiliation:
School of Information, University of Michigan, 105 South State Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2112, USA

Abstract

Temporary changes in biological state, such as hunger, can impact decision making differently for men and women. Food scarcity is correlated with a host of negative economic outcomes. Two explanations for this correlation are that hunger affects economic preferences directly or that hunger creates a mindset that focuses on scarcity management to the detriment of other decisions. To test these predictions, we conduct a lab-in-the-field experiment in a health screening clinic in Shanghai, recruiting participants who finish their annual physical exam either before or after they have eaten breakfast. We compare the hungry and sated groups on their risk, time and generosity preferences as well as their cognitive performance. Our results show that men and women respond to hunger in opposite directions, thus hunger reduces the gender gap in decision quality, risk aversion and cognitive performance, but creates one in generosity. Finally, we examine several biomarkers and find that higher blood lipid levels are correlated with greater choice inconsistency, risk aversion and generosity. We contribute to emerging insights on the biological foundations for economic preferences and outcomes.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2018 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-018-9589-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

We would like to thank Colin Camerer, Soo Hong Chew, Uri Gneezy, Muriel Niederle, Georg Weizsäcker, Maytal Shabat-Simon, and participants at Michigan (Ross), the Jerusalem Conference on the Typologies of Bounded Rationality (2015), the International ESA meetings (Sydney, Australia, 2015) for helpful comments, Jim Andreoni, Peter Kuhn and Charles Sprenger for sharing their data and code with us, as well as Carrie Wenjing Xu, Linfeng Li and Kurtis Drodge for excellent research assistance. We thank two anonymous referees and the Editor, Lata Gangadharan, for their thoughtful and constructive comments which significantly improved the paper. The financial support from the National Science Foundation through Grant No. BCS-1111019 to Chen is gratefully acknowledged. The research has been approved by the University of Michigan IRB.

References

Aarøe, L, & Petersen, MB (2013). Hunger games: Fluctuations in blood glucose levels influence support for social welfare. Psychological Science, 24(12), 25502556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aarts, H, Dijksterhuis, A, & Vries, P (2001). On the psychology of drinking: Being thirsty and perceptually ready. British Journal of Psychology, 92(4), 631642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ai, C, & Norton, EC (2003). Interaction terms in logit and probit models. Economics Letters, 80(1), 123129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J., Kuhn, M. A., & Sprenger, C. (2013). On measuring time preferences. Technical report, National Bureau of Economic Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J, Kuhn, MA, & Sprenger, C (2015). Measuring time preferences: A comparison of experimental methods. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 116, 451464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J, & Miller, J (2002). Giving according to GARP: An experimental test of the consistency of preferences for altruism. Econometrica, 70(2), 737753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J, & Sprenger, C (2012). Estimating time preferences from convex budgets. The American Economic Review, 102, 33333356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J, & Vesterlund, L (2001). Which is the fair sex? Gender differences in altruism. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 293312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashraf, N, Karlan, D, & Yin, W (2006). Tying Odysseus to the mast: Evidence from a commitment savings product in the Philippines. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121, 635672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashton, L. (2014). Hunger Games: Does Hunger and cognitive fatigue affect time preferences? Available at SSRN 2538740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Askew, E. W., Munro, I., Sharp, M. A., Siegel, S., Popper, R.,, & Rose, M. S., et al. (1987). Nutritional status and physical and mental performance of special operations soldiers consuming the ration, lightweight, or the meal, ready-to-eat military field ration during a 30-day field training exercise. Technical report, DTIC Document 1987.Google Scholar
Baumeister, RF, & Vohs, KD (2007). Self-regulation, ego depletion, and motivation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 115128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beasley, JM, Ange, BA, Anderson, CAM, Miller, ER, Holbrook, JT, & Appel, LJ (2009). Characteristics associated with fasting appetite hormones (obestatin, ghrelin, and leptin). Obesity, 17(2), 349354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, D, & Parker, PY (1998). Breakfast, blood glucose, and cognition. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 67(4), 772S778S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertrand, M Ashenfelter, O, & Card, D (2010). New perspectives on gender. Handbook of labor economics, Amsterdam: Elsevier 15431590.Google Scholar
Blank, RM, & Barr, MS (2009). Insufficient funds: Savings, assets, credit, and banking among low-income households, New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Caraco, T, Martindale, S, & Whittam, TS (1980). An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behaviour, 28(3), 820830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, JF, Kaiser, KA, Franks, SF, Deere, C, & Caffrey, JL (2007). Influence of BMI and gender on postprandial hormone responses. Obesity, 15(12), 29742983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carvalho, LS, Meier, S, & Wang, SW (2016). Poverty and economic decision-making: Evidence from changes in financial resources at payday. The American Economic Review, 106(2), 260–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassar, A, Wordofa, F, & Zhang, YJ (2016). Competing for the benefit of offspring eliminates the gender gap in competitiveness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(19), 52015205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, SL (2015). Comment on ‘risk preferences are not time preferences’: On the elicitation of time preference under conditions of risk. The American Economic Review, 105(7), 22422260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornier, M-A, Salzberg, AK, Endly, DC, Bessesen, DH, & Tregellas, JR (2010). Sex-based differences in the behavioral and neuronal responses to food. Physiology and Behavior, 99(4), 538543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croson, RTA, & Gneezy, U (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danziger, S, Levav, J, & Avnaim-Pesso, L (2011). Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(17), 68896892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dave, C, Eckel, CC, Johnson, CA, & Rojas, C (2010). Eliciting risk preferences: When is simple better?. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 41(3), 219243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Parigi, A, Chen, K, Gautier, J-F, Salbe, AD, Pratley, RE, Ravussin, E, Reiman, EM, & Tataranni, A (2002). Sex differences in the human brain’s response to hunger and satiation. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 75(6), 10171022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devore, E., Ridker, P., & Grodstein, F. (2004). O3-01-02 Plasma cholesterol levels and cognitive function in aging women. Neurobiology of Aging, 25(Suppl 2), S52 (Abstracts from the 9th International Conference on Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, DL, McElroy, T, & Stroh, N (2014). The impact of glucose on Bayesian versus heuristic-based decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 7(4), 237247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T, Falk, A, Huffman, D, & Sunde, U (2010). Are risk aversion and impatience related to cognitive ability?. The American Economic Review, 100(3), 1238–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T, Falk, A, Huffman, D, Sunde, U, Schupp, J, & Wagner, GG (2011). Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 9(3), 522550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, CC, Grossman, PJ Plott, CR, & Smith, VL (2008). Differences in the economic decisions of men and women: Experimental evidence. Handbook of experimental economics results, Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Epper, T, & Fehr-Duda, H (2015). Comment on ‘risk preferences are not time preferences’: Balancing on a budget line. The American Economic Review, 105(7), 22612271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonseca-Azevedo, K, & Herculano-Houzel, S (2012). Metabolic constraint imposes tradeoff between body size and number of brain neurons in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(45), 1857118576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Food and Agriculture Organization. (2015). The state of food insecurity in the world.Google Scholar
Frank, S, Laharnar, N, Kullmann, S, Veit, R, Canova, C, Hegner, YL Sved, A F, & Moran, T H (2010). Processing of food pictures: Influence of hunger, gender and calorie content. Brain research: Neural mechanisms of ingestive behaviour and obesity, Amsterdam: Elseiver 159166.Google Scholar
Frederick, S (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 2542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederick, S, Loewenstein, G, & O’Donoghue, T (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 351401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gailliot, MT, Baumeister, RF, Nathan DeWall, C, Maner, JK, Ashby Plant, E, Tice, DM, Brewer, LE, & Schmeichel, BJ (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2), 325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guiso, L, & Paiella, M (2008). Risk aversion, wealth, and background risk. Journal of the European Economic Association, 6(6), 11091150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haier, RJ, & Benbow, CP (1995). Sex differences and lateralization in temporal lobe glucose metabolism during mathematical reasoning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 11(4), 405414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I., & Rutström, E. E. (2013). Identifying time preferences with experiments: Comment. Center for the Economic Analysis of Risk, Working Paper, 9.Google Scholar
Haushofer, J, & Fehr, E (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science, 344(6186), 862867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, GW, Gavin, JR III, Hinderliter, JM, Hagberg, JM, Bloomfield, SA, & Holloszy, JO (1983). Effects of exercise and lack of exercise on glucose tolerance and insulin sensitivity. Journal of Applied Physiology, 55(2), 512517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hersch, J (1996). Smoking, seat belts and other risky consumer decisions: Differences by gender and race. Managerial and Decision Economics, 17(5), 471481.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, CA, & Laury, SK (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. The American Economic Review, 92(5), 16441655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, S, & Petrie, R (2009). Learning from mistakes: What do inconsistent choices over risk tell us?. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38(2), 143158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability, Westport: Praeger.Google Scholar
Jianakoplos, NA, & Bernasek, A (1998). Are women more risk averse?. Economic Inquiry, 36, 620630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalmijn, S, Foley, D, White, L, Burchfiel, CM, Curb, JD, Petrovitch, H, Ross, GW, Havlik, RJ, & Launer, LJ (2000). Metabolic cardiovascular syndrome and risk of dementia in Japanese–American elderly men: The Honolulu-Asia aging study. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology, 20(10), 22552260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, SJ, & Hofer, TP (1994). Socioeconomic disparities in preventive care persist despite universal coverage: Breast and cervical cancer screening in Ontario and the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272(7), 530534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kearney, MS (2005). State lotteries and consumer behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 89(11), 22692299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, B-J, Carlson, OD, Jang, H-J, Elahi, D, Berry, C, & Egan, JM (2005). Peptide YY is secreted after oral glucose administration in a gender-specific manner. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 90(12), 66656671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kivipelto, M, Helkala, E-L, Laakso, MP, Hänninen, T, Hallikainen, M, Alhainen, K, Soininen, H, Tuomilehto, J, & Nissinen, A (2001). Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimer’s disease in later life: Longitudinal, population based study. BMJ, 322(7300), 14471451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, MA, Kuhn, P, & Villeval, MC (2017). Decision-environment effects on intertemporal financial choices: How relevant are resource-depletion models?. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 137, 7289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, DJ, Thavikulwat, AC, & Glimcher, PW (2013). State Dependent valuation: The effect of deprivation on risk preferences. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e53978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mani, A, Mullainathan, S, Shafir, E, & Zhao, J (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341(6149), 976980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miao, B, & Zhong, S (2015). Comment on ‘Risk preferences are not time preferences’: Separating risk and time preference. The American Economic Review, 105(7), 22722286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullainathan, S, & Shafir, E (2013). Scarcity: Why having too little means so much, London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Niederle, M Kagel, JH, & Roth, AE (2016). Gender. The handbook of experimental economics, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Prasad, K, & Salmon, TC (2013). Self selection and market power in risk sharing contracts. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 90, 7186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radel, R, & Clément-Guillotin, C (2012). Evidence of motivational influences in early visual perception hunger modulates conscious access. Psychological Science, 23(3), 232234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schofield, H. (2014). The economic costs of low caloric intake: Evidence from India. Harvard University (Unpublished).Google Scholar
Shah, AK, Mullainathan, S, & Shafir, E (2012). Some consequences of having too little. Science, 338(6107), 682685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Symmonds, M, Emmanuel, JJ, Drew, ME, Batterham, RL, & Dolan, RJ (2010). Metabolic state alters economic decision making under risk in humans. PLOS ONE, 5(6), 17 06.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanaka, T, Camerer, CF, & Nguyen, Q (2010). Risk and time preferences: Linking experimental and household survey data from Vietnam. The American Economic Review, 100(1), 557571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, G-J, Volkow, ND, Telang, F, Jayne, M, Ma, Y, Pradhan, K, Zhu, W, Wong, CT, Thanos, PK, Geliebter, A, Biegon, A, & Fowler, JS (2009). Evidence of gender differences in the ability to inhibit brain activation elicited by food stimulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(4), 12491254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, W, Zhao, D, & Wu, Z (2001). Study of triglyceride distribution aged 35–64, and their association with other cardiovascular disease risk factors in 11 provinces. Chinese Journal of Epidemiology, 22, 2629.Google ScholarPubMed
Whitmer, RA, Sidney, S, Selby, J, Johnston, SC, & Yaffe, K (2005). Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late life. Neurology, 64(2), 277281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamada, H, Tymula, A, Louie, K, & Glimcher, PW (2013). Thirst-dependent risk preferences in monkeys identify a primitive form of wealth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(39), 1578815793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Chen et al. supplementary material

Online Appendices for Hunger and the Gender Gap
Download Chen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 5 MB