Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-dkgms Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T10:44:06.724Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effects of externalities and framing on bribery in a petty corruption experiment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Abigail Barr*
Affiliation:
Centre for the Study of African Economies, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Danila Serra*
Affiliation:
Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA

Abstract

Using a simple one-shot bribery game simulating petty corruption exchanges, we find evidence of a negative externality effect and a framing effect. When the losses suffered by third parties due to a bribe being offered and accepted are high and the game is presented as a petty corruption scenario instead of in abstract terms bribes are less likely to be offered. Higher negative externalities are also associated with less bribe acceptance. However, framing has no effect on bribe acceptance, indicating that the issue of artificiality may be of particular importance in bribery experiments.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Economic Science Association 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-009-9225-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Abbink, K. (2005). Fair salaries and the moral costs of corruption. In Proceedings of the conference on cognitive economics, Sofia.Google Scholar
Abbink, K., & Hennig Schmidt, H. (2006). Neutral versus loaded instructions in a bribery experiment. Experimental Economics, 9(2), 103121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbink, K., Irlenbusch, B., & Renner, E. (2002). An experimental bribery game. Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization, 18(2), 428454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alatas, V., Cameron, L., Chaudhuri, A., Erkal, N., & Gangadharan, L. (2009). Subject pool effects in a corruption experiment: a comparison of Indonesian public servants and Indonesian students. Experimental Economics, 1, 113132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J. (1995). Warm-glow versus cold-prickle: the effects of positive and negative framing on cooperation in experiments. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardsley, N. (2005). Experimental economics and the artificiality of alteration. Journal of Economic Methodology, 12, 239251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilotkach, V. (2006). A tax evasion—bribery game: experimental evidence from Ukraine. European Journal of Comparative Economics, 3(1), 3149.Google Scholar
Brosig, J., Weimann, J., & Yang, C.-L. (2003). The hot versus cold effect in a simple bargaining experiment. Experimental Economics, 6(1), 7590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branas-Garza, P. (2007). Promoting helping behavior with framing in dictator games. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(4), 477486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandts, J., & Charness, G. (2000). Hot vs. cold: sequential experimental games. Experimental Economics, 2, 227238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, L., Chaudhuri, A., Erkal, N., & Gangadharan, L. (2009). Propensities to engage in and punish corrupt behavior: experimental evidence from Australia, India, Indonesia and Singapore. Journal of Public Economics, 93(7-8), 843851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cason, T., & Mui, V. (1998). Social influence in the sequential dictator game. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 42, 248265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cookson, R. (2000). Framing effects in public goods experiments. Experimental Economics, 3, 5579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, D. J., Kagel, J., Lo, W., & Gu, Q. L. (1999). Gaming against managers in incentive systems: experimental results with Chinese students and Chinese managers. American Economics Review, 89(4), 781804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, Ph. J. (1996). Altruism in anonymous dictator games. Games and Economic Behavior, 16, 181191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & List, J. (2004). The hidden costs and returns of incentives—trust and trustworthiness among CEOs. Journal of the European Economic Association, 2(5), 743771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Rockenbach, B. (2003). Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism. Nature, 422(13), 137140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114, 817868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbacher, U., Gächter, S., & Fehr, E. (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71, 397404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Güth, W., Huck, S., & Mueller, W. (2001). The relevance of equal splits in ultimatum games. Games and Economic Behavior, 37, 161169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, V., Samuels, S., & Ross, L. (2004). The name of the game: predictive power of reputations vs. situational labels in determining prisoner's dilemma game moves. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 11751185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loomes, G. (1999). Some lessons from past experiments and some challenges for the future. Economic Journal, 109, F35F45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oxoby, R. J., & McLeish, K. N. (2004). Sequential decision and strategy vector methods in ultimatum bargaining: evidence on the strength of other regarding behaviour. Economic Letters, 84, 399405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rege, M., & Telle, K. (2004). The impact of social approval and framing on cooperation in public goods situations. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 16251644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naive realism in everyday life: implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. In Reed, E.S., Turiel, E. & Brown, T. (Eds.), Values and knowledge (pp. 103135). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schotter, A., Weigelt, K., & Wilson, C. (1994). A laboratory investigation of multi-person rationality and presentation effects. Games and Economic Behavior, 6, 445468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svensson, J. (2005). Eight questions about corruption. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(3), 1942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
International, Transparency (2006). Global Corruption Report 2006.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Barr and Serra supplementary material

Supplementary materials
Download Barr and Serra supplementary material(File)
File 35.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Barr and Serra supplementary material

Tables 1 and 2
Download Barr and Serra supplementary material(File)
File 95.2 KB