Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-6tpvb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-15T16:04:21.659Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social preferences in the online laboratory: a randomized experiment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Jérôme Hergueux
Affiliation:
Institute of Political Studies, University of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France Department of Economics, Sciences Po, Paris, France Berkman Center for Internet & Society, Harvard University, Cambridge, USA
Nicolas Jacquemet*
Affiliation:
Université de Lorraine (BETA), 13 Place Carnot, 54035 Nancy, France Paris School of Economics, Paris, France

Abstract

Internet is a very attractive technology for the implementation of experiments, both in order to obtain larger and more diverse samples and as a field of economic research in its own right. This paper reports on an experiment performed both online and in the laboratory, designed to strengthen the internal validity of decisions elicited over the Internet. We use the same subject pool, the same monetary stakes and the same decision interface, and control the assignment of subjects between the Internet and a traditional university laboratory. We apply the comparison to the elicitation of social preferences in a Public Good game, a dictator game, an ultimatum bargaining game and a trust game, coupled with an elicitation of risk aversion. This comparison concludes in favor of the reliability of behaviors elicited through the Internet. We moreover find a strong overall parallelism in the preferences elicited in the two settings. The paper also reports some quantitative differences in the point estimates, which always go in the direction of more other-regarding decisions from online subjects. This observation challenges either the predictions of social distance theory or the generally assumed increased social distance in internet interactions.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2014 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-014-9400-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Akerlof, GA (1997). Social distance and social decisions. Econometrica, 65(5), 10051027. 10.2307/2171877CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amir, O, Rand, DG, & Gal, YK (2012). Economic games on the Internet: The effect of $1 stakes. PLoS ONE, 7(2), 14. 10.1371/journal.pone.0031461CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderhub, V, Müller, R, & Schmidt, C (2001). Design and evaluation of an economic experiment via the Internet. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 46(2), 227247. 10.1016/S0167-2681(01)00195-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bainbridge, WS (2007). The scientific research potential of virtual worlds. Science, 317(5837), 472476. 10.1126/science.1146930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bland, JM, & Altman, DG (1995). Multiple significance tests: The Bonferroni method. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 310(6973), 170 10.1136/bmj.310.6973.170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charness, G, Haruvy, E, & Sonsino, D (2007). Social distance and reciprocity: An Internet experiment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 63(1), 88103. 10.1016/j.jebo.2005.04.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chesney, T, Chuah, S-H, & Hoffmann, R (2009). Virtual world experimentation: An exploratory study. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 618635. 10.1016/j.jebo.2009.05.026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, DJ, & Saral, KJ (2013). Entrepreneurship and team participation: An experimental study. European Economic Review, 59, 126140. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2013.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T et al., (2011). Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 9(3), 522550. 10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, CC, & Wilson, RK (2006). Internet cautions: Experimental games with Internet partners. Experimental Economics, 9(1), 5366. 10.1007/s10683-006-4307-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E, & Camerer, CF (2004). Measuring social norms and preferences using experimental games: A guide for social scientists. Foundations of Human Sociality, 1(9), 5596.Google Scholar
Fiedler, M, & Haruvy, E (2009). The lab versus the virtual lab and virtual field—An experimental investigation of trust games with communication. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(2), 716724. 10.1016/j.jebo.2009.07.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, M, Haruvy, E, & Li, SX (2011). Social distance in a virtual world experiment. Games and Economic Behavior, 72(2), 400426. 10.1016/j.geb.2010.09.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbacher, U, Gächter, S, & Fehr, E (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71(3), 397404. 10.1016/S0165-1765(01)00394-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glaeser, EL et al., (2000). Measuring Trust. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 811846. 10.1162/003355300554926CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greif, A (2006). Institutions and the path to the modern economy: Lessons from medieval trade, New York: Cambridge University Press 10.1017/CBO9780511791307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greiner, B (2004). An online recruitment system for economic experiments, Germany: University Library of Munich.Google Scholar
Henrich, J, Heine, SJ, & Norenzayan, A (2010). The weirdest people in the world?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. 10.1017/S0140525X0999152XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, E, McCabe, K, & Smith, VL (1996). Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games. The American Economic Review, 86(3), 653660.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M., & Morgan, J. (2011). Who’s Naughty? Who’s Nice? Social Preferences in Online Industries. UC Berkeley Working Paper.Google Scholar
Holt, CA, & Laury, SK (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. The American Economic Review, 92(5), 16441655. 10.1257/000282802762024700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horton, JJ, Rand, DG, & Zeckhauser, RJ (2011). The online laboratory: Conducting experiments in a real labor market. Experimental Economics, 14(3), 399425. 10.1007/s10683-011-9273-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. The American Economic Review, 93(5), 14491475. 10.1257/000282803322655392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lotito, G, Migheli, M, & Ortona, G (2013). Is cooperation instinctive? Evidence from the response times in a public goods game. Journal of Bioeconomics, 15(2), 123133. 10.1007/s10818-012-9141-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piovesan, M., & Wengstrom, E. (2009). Fast or fair? A study of response times. Economics Letters, 105(2), 193196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rand, DG, Greene, JD, & Nowak, MA (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489(7416), 427430. 10.1038/nature11467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resnick, P et al., (2006). The value of reputation on eBay: A controlled experiment. Experimental Economics, 9(2), 79101. 10.1007/s10683-006-4309-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubinstein, A (2007). Instinctive and cognitive reasoning: A study of response times. The Economic Journal, 117(523), 12431259. 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02081.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selten, R, & Sauermann, H (1967). Die strategiemethode zur erforschung des eingeschrankt rationalen verhaltens im rahmen eines oligopol experiments. Beitrage zur Experimentellen Wirtschaftsforschung, Tubingen: J.C.B. Mohr 136168.Google Scholar
Shavit, T, Sonsino, D, & Benzion, U (2001). A comparative study of lotteries-evaluation in class and on the Web. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22(4), 483491. 10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00048-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Hergueux and Jacquemet supplementary material

Appendix – Companion treatments on the effect of the Internet specific differences in design
Download Hergueux and Jacquemet supplementary material(File)
File 199.6 KB